February 21, 2024

Why Writing by Hand Is Better for Memory and Learning

Engaging the fine motor system to produce letters by hand has positive effects on learning and memory

By Charlotte Hu

Child laying on his bed writing.

Studies continue to show pluses to writing by hand.

Image Source/Getty Images

Handwriting notes in class might seem like an anachronism as smartphones and other digital technology subsume every aspect of learning across schools and universities. But a steady stream of research continues to suggest that taking notes the traditional way—with pen and paper or even stylus and tablet—is still the best way to learn, especially for young children. And now scientists are finally zeroing in on why.

A recent study in Frontiers in Psychology monitored brain activity in students taking notes and found that those writing by hand had higher levels of electrical activity across a wide range of interconnected brain regions responsible for movement, vision, sensory processing and memory. The findings add to a growing body of evidence that has many experts speaking up about the importance of teaching children to handwrite words and draw pictures.

Differences in Brain Activity

On supporting science journalism.

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing . By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.

The new research, by Audrey van der Meer and Ruud van der Weel at the Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), builds on a foundational 2014 study . That work suggested that people taking notes by computer were typing without thinking, says van der Meer , a professor of neuropsychology at NTNU. “It’s very tempting to type down everything that the lecturer is saying,” she says. “It kind of goes in through your ears and comes out through your fingertips, but you don’t process the incoming information.” But when taking notes by hand, it’s often impossible to write everything down; students have to actively pay attention to the incoming information and process it—prioritize it, consolidate it and try to relate it to things they’ve learned before. This conscious action of building onto existing knowledge can make it easier to stay engaged and grasp new concepts .

To understand specific brain activity differences during the two note-taking approaches, the NTNU researchers tweaked the 2014 study’s basic setup. They sewed electrodes into a hairnet with 256 sensors that recorded the brain activity of 36 students as they wrote or typed 15 words from the game Pictionary that were displayed on a screen.

When students wrote the words by hand, the sensors picked up widespread connectivity across many brain regions. Typing, however, led to minimal activity, if any, in the same areas. Handwriting activated connection patterns spanning visual regions, regions that receive and process sensory information and the motor cortex. The latter handles body movement and sensorimotor integration, which helps the brain use environmental inputs to inform a person’s next action.

“When you are typing, the same simple movement of your fingers is involved in producing every letter, whereas when you’re writing by hand, you immediately feel that the bodily feeling of producing A is entirely different from producing a B,” van der Meer says. She notes that children who have learned to read and write by tapping on a digital tablet “often have difficulty distinguishing letters that look a lot like each other or that are mirror images of each other, like the b and the d.”

Reinforcing Memory and Learning Pathways

Sophia Vinci-Booher , an assistant professor of educational neuroscience at Vanderbilt University who was not involved in the new study, says its findings are exciting and consistent with past research. “You can see that in tasks that really lock the motor and sensory systems together, such as in handwriting, there’s this really clear tie between this motor action being accomplished and the visual and conceptual recognition being created,” she says. “As you’re drawing a letter or writing a word, you’re taking this perceptual understanding of something and using your motor system to create it.” That creation is then fed back into the visual system, where it’s processed again—strengthening the connection between an action and the images or words associated with it. It’s similar to imagining something and then creating it: when you materialize something from your imagination (by writing it, drawing it or building it), this reinforces the imagined concept and helps it stick in your memory.

The phenomenon of boosting memory by producing something tangible has been well studied. Previous research has found that when people are asked to write, draw or act out a word that they’re reading, they have to focus more on what they’re doing with the received information. Transferring verbal information to a different form, such as a written format, also involves activating motor programs in the brain to create a specific sequence of hand motions, explains Yadurshana Sivashankar , a cognitive neuroscience graduate student at the University of Waterloo in Ontario who studies movement and memory. But handwriting requires more of the brain’s motor programs than typing. “When you’re writing the word ‘the,’ the actual movements of the hand relate to the structures of the word to some extent,” says Sivashankar, who was not involved in the new study.

For example, participants in a 2021 study by Sivashankar memorized a list of action verbs more accurately if they performed the corresponding action than if they performed an unrelated action or none at all. “Drawing information and enacting information is helpful because you have to think about information and you have to produce something that’s meaningful,” she says. And by transforming the information, you pave and deepen these interconnections across the brain’s vast neural networks, making it “much easier to access that information.”

The Importance of Handwriting Lessons for Kids

Across many contexts, studies have shown that kids appear to learn better when they’re asked to produce letters or other visual items using their fingers and hands in a coordinated way—one that can’t be replicated by clicking a mouse or tapping buttons on a screen or keyboard. Vinci-Booher’s research has also found that the action of handwriting appears to engage different brain regions at different levels than other standard learning experiences, such as reading or observing. Her work has also shown that handwriting improves letter recognition in preschool children, and the effects of learning through writing “last longer than other learning experiences that might engage attention at a similar level,” Vinci-Booher says. Additionally, she thinks it’s possible that engaging the motor system is how children learn how to break “ mirror invariance ” (registering mirror images as identical) and begin to decipher things such as the difference between the lowercase b and p.

Vinci-Booher says the new study opens up bigger questions about the way we learn, such as how brain region connections change over time and when these connections are most important in learning. She and other experts say, however, that the new findings don’t mean technology is a disadvantage in the classroom. Laptops, smartphones and other such devices can be more efficient for writing essays or conducting research and can offer more equitable access to educational resources. Problems occur when people rely on technology too much , Sivashankar says. People are increasingly delegating thought processes to digital devices, an act called “ cognitive offloading ”—using smartphones to remember tasks, taking a photo instead of memorizing information or depending on a GPS to navigate. “It’s helpful, but we think the constant offloading means it’s less work for the brain,” Sivashankar says. “If we’re not actively using these areas, then they are going to deteriorate over time, whether it’s memory or motor skills.”

Van der Meer says some officials in Norway are inching toward implementing completely digital schools . She claims first grade teachers there have told her their incoming students barely know how to hold a pencil now—which suggests they weren’t coloring pictures or assembling puzzles in nursery school. Van der Meer says they’re missing out on opportunities that can help stimulate their growing brains.

“I think there’s a very strong case for engaging children in drawing and handwriting activities, especially in preschool and kindergarten when they’re first learning about letters,” Vinci-Booher says. “There’s something about engaging the fine motor system and production activities that really impacts learning.”

A version of this article entitled “Hands-on” was adapted for inclusion in the May 2024 issue of Scientific American.

  • Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience
  • Neuroplasticity and Development
  • Research Topics

Learning and Memory 2023 - Volume II

Total Downloads

Total Views and Downloads

About this Research Topic

Given the success of the Learning and Memory Research Topic and the rapidly evolving subject area, we are pleased to announce the launch of Volume II. The field of learning and memory has evolved rapidly over ...

Keywords : #LEARNMEM2023, Learning, Memory, Cognition, Hippocampus, Aging, Synpatic, Plasticity, Place cells, Systems Neuroscience, Grid cells

Important Note : All contributions to this Research Topic must be within the scope of the section and journal to which they are submitted, as defined in their mission statements. Frontiers reserves the right to guide an out-of-scope manuscript to a more suitable section or journal at any stage of peer review.

Topic Editors

Topic coordinators, recent articles, submission deadlines.

Submission closed.

Participating Journals

Total views.

  • Demographics

No records found

total views article views downloads topic views

Top countries

Top referring sites, about frontiers research topics.

With their unique mixes of varied contributions from Original Research to Review Articles, Research Topics unify the most influential researchers, the latest key findings and historical advances in a hot research area! Find out more on how to host your own Frontiers Research Topic or contribute to one as an author.

American Psychological Association Logo

Learning and memory

elementary school students looking at a book

Learning and memory are closely related concepts. Learning is the acquisition of skill or knowledge, while memory is the expression of what you’ve acquired.

Another difference is the speed with which the two things happen. If you acquire the new skill or knowledge slowly and laboriously, that’s learning. If acquisition occurs instantly, that’s making a memory.

Adapted from the Encyclopedia of Psychology

Resources from APA

Speaking of Psychology: What déjà vu can teach us about memory, with Chris Moulin, PhD

What déjà vu can teach us about memory

Chris Moulin, PhD, talks about why déjà vu happens.

Speaking of Psychology: You can learn new things at any age, with Rachel Wu, PhD

You can learn new things at any age

Rachel Wu, PhD, talks about why lifelong learning matters.

Speaking of Psychology: How music, memory and emotion are connected, with Elizabeth Margulis, PhD

How music, memory and emotion are connected

Elizabeth Margulis, PhD, talks about how music, memory, emotion and imagination intertwine.

Speaking of Psychology: How to learn from regret, with Robert Leahy, PhD

How to learn from regret

Robert Leahy, PhD, talks about the difference between productive and unproductive regret.

More resources about learning and memory

Keep Your Wits About You

Becoming Brilliant

Study Like a Champ

Essentials of Conditioning and Learning 5th

Bilingualism Across the Lifespan

Magination Press children’s books

Cover of So Many Smarts! (medium)

So Many Smarts!

Cover of Lucy in the City (medium)

Lucy in the City

Cover of Mapping My Day (medium)

Mapping My Day

Cover of School Made Easier (medium)

School Made Easier

Journal special issues

Metacognition

Comparative Cognition

The Role of Historical Change for Adult Development and Aging

Neurodevelopment

How Psychological Science Can Help Improve Our Classrooms

  • Advanced search

Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

The consolidation of newly learned movements depends upon somatosensory cortex in humans

  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: [email protected]
  • Info & Metrics

Studies using magnetic brain stimulation indicate the involvement of somatosensory regions in the acquisition and retention of newly learned movements. Recent work found an impairment in motor memory when retention was tested shortly after the application of continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) to primary somatosensory cortex, compared to stimulation of primary motor cortex or a control zone. This finding, that somatosensory cortex is involved in motor memory retention whereas motor cortex is not, if confirmed, could alter our understanding of human motor learning. It would indicate that plasticity in sensory systems underlies newly learned movements, which is different than the commonly held view that adaptation learning involves updates to a motor controller. Here we provide a test of this idea. Participants (both sexes) trained in a visuomotor adaptation task, with visual feedback gradually shifted. Following adaptation, cTBS was applied either to M1, S1, or an occipital cortex control area. Participants were tested for retention 24h later. It was observed that S1 stimulation led to reduced retention of prior learning, compared to stimulation of M1 or the control area (with no significant difference between M1 and control). In a further control, cTBS was applied to S1 following training with unrotated feedback, in which no learning occurred. This had no effect on movement in the retention test indicating the effects of cTBS to S1 on movement are learning-specific. The findings are consistent with the S1 participation in the encoding of learning related changes to movements and in the retention of human motor memory.

Significance Statement This paper uses transcranial magnetic stimulation which is applied after sensorimotor learning to disrupt brain areas that might be involved in the retention of newly learned movements. Tests of retention are conducted 24h after learning to allow for motor memory stabilization. It is found that disruption of somatosensory cortex produces an impairment in retention whereas disruption of motor cortex does not. Thus, somatosensory cortex is part of a circuit that encodes newly learned movements. The present study finds that primary motor cortex is not part of this circuit. Thus, plasticity in sensory systems underlies newly learned movements, which is different than the commonly held view that learning involves updates to a motor controller.

This work was supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR PJT 165987 and CIHR PJT 183970) to D.J.O.

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

SfN exclusive license .

Member Log In

Log in using your username and password, purchase access.

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Citation Manager Formats

  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager

Reddit logo

  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

Responses to this article, jump to comment:, related articles, cited by..., more in this toc section, research articles.

  • Neural activity differentiates novel and learned event boundaries
  • A comparison of rapid rule-learning strategies in humans and monkeys

Behavioral/Cognitive

Help | Advanced Search

Computer Science > Machine Learning

Title: qlora: efficient finetuning of quantized llms.

Abstract: We present QLoRA, an efficient finetuning approach that reduces memory usage enough to finetune a 65B parameter model on a single 48GB GPU while preserving full 16-bit finetuning task performance. QLoRA backpropagates gradients through a frozen, 4-bit quantized pretrained language model into Low Rank Adapters~(LoRA). Our best model family, which we name Guanaco, outperforms all previous openly released models on the Vicuna benchmark, reaching 99.3% of the performance level of ChatGPT while only requiring 24 hours of finetuning on a single GPU. QLoRA introduces a number of innovations to save memory without sacrificing performance: (a) 4-bit NormalFloat (NF4), a new data type that is information theoretically optimal for normally distributed weights (b) double quantization to reduce the average memory footprint by quantizing the quantization constants, and (c) paged optimziers to manage memory spikes. We use QLoRA to finetune more than 1,000 models, providing a detailed analysis of instruction following and chatbot performance across 8 instruction datasets, multiple model types (LLaMA, T5), and model scales that would be infeasible to run with regular finetuning (e.g. 33B and 65B parameter models). Our results show that QLoRA finetuning on a small high-quality dataset leads to state-of-the-art results, even when using smaller models than the previous SoTA. We provide a detailed analysis of chatbot performance based on both human and GPT-4 evaluations showing that GPT-4 evaluations are a cheap and reasonable alternative to human evaluation. Furthermore, we find that current chatbot benchmarks are not trustworthy to accurately evaluate the performance levels of chatbots. A lemon-picked analysis demonstrates where Guanaco fails compared to ChatGPT. We release all of our models and code, including CUDA kernels for 4-bit training.
Comments: Extended NeurIPS submission
Subjects: Machine Learning (cs.LG)
Cite as: [cs.LG]
  (or [cs.LG] for this version)
  Focus to learn more arXiv-issued DOI via DataCite

Submission history

Access paper:.

  • Other Formats

license icon

References & Citations

  • Google Scholar
  • Semantic Scholar

6 blog links

Bibtex formatted citation.

BibSonomy logo

Bibliographic and Citation Tools

Code, data and media associated with this article, recommenders and search tools.

  • Institution

arXivLabs: experimental projects with community collaborators

arXivLabs is a framework that allows collaborators to develop and share new arXiv features directly on our website.

Both individuals and organizations that work with arXivLabs have embraced and accepted our values of openness, community, excellence, and user data privacy. arXiv is committed to these values and only works with partners that adhere to them.

Have an idea for a project that will add value for arXiv's community? Learn more about arXivLabs .

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

diagnostics-logo

Article Menu

learning and memory research paper

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Deep learning for alzheimer’s disease prediction: a comprehensive review.

learning and memory research paper

Share and Cite

Malik, I.; Iqbal, A.; Gu, Y.H.; Al-antari, M.A. Deep Learning for Alzheimer’s Disease Prediction: A Comprehensive Review. Diagnostics 2024 , 14 , 1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/diagnostics14121281

Malik I, Iqbal A, Gu YH, Al-antari MA. Deep Learning for Alzheimer’s Disease Prediction: A Comprehensive Review. Diagnostics . 2024; 14(12):1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/diagnostics14121281

Malik, Isra, Ahmed Iqbal, Yeong Hyeon Gu, and Mugahed A. Al-antari. 2024. "Deep Learning for Alzheimer’s Disease Prediction: A Comprehensive Review" Diagnostics 14, no. 12: 1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/diagnostics14121281

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

learning and memory research paper

We’re a group of 3,000 researchers inventing what’s next in computing at labs across the world. Learn more about us and our work below.

  • Yorktown Heights

Europe, Middle East, and Africa

  • United Kingdom

Ashley Evans

Ashley Evans

Manish Kesarwani

Manish Kesarwani

Serpil Kiokekli

Serpil Kiokekli

Marcel Kossel

Marcel Kossel

Nelson Felix

Nelson Felix

Ran Iwamoto

Ran Iwamoto

Achintya Kundu

Achintya Kundu

Elger Anne Vlieg

Elger Anne Vlieg

Jennifer Liang

Jennifer Liang

Mark Wegman

Mark Wegman

Fabian Thiemann

Fabian Thiemann

Brent Anderson

Brent Anderson

Antonio Mezzacapo

Antonio Mezzacapo

Surya Shravan Kumar Sajja

Surya Shravan Kumar Sajja

Wei Sun

Abbas Rahimi

Vadim Elisseev

Vadim Elisseev

Joao Lucas de Sousa Almeida

Joao Lucas de Sousa Almeida

Dechao Guo

Filip Michal Janicki

Haoxiang Qiu

Haoxiang Qiu

Riko Kudo

Pier Andrea Francese

Nikita Janakarajan

Nikita Janakarajan

Six Books That Explain The History And Meaning Of Juneteenth

Giving compass' take:.

  • Wake Forest University humanities professor Corey D. B. Walker recommends six readings to help contextualize and highlight why we observe Juneteenth.
  • How has the observance of Juneteenth helped orient donors to understanding the history of Black communities' economic mobility in the United States?
  • Learn how funders can affirm and support Black female leadership. 

What is Giving Compass?

We connect donors to learning resources and ways to support community-led solutions. Learn more about us .

After decades of being celebrated at mostly the local level, Juneteenth – the long-standing holiday that commemorates the arrival of news of emancipation and freedom to enslaved Black people in Galveston, Texas, in 1865 – became a federal holiday in 2021. In honor of this year’s Juneteenth, The Conversation reached out to Wake Forest University humanities professor Corey D. B. Walker for a list of readings that can help people better understand the history and meaning of the observance. Below, Walker recommends six books.

‘On Juneteenth’

Combining history and memoir, Annette Gordon-Reed’s “ On Juneteenth ” offers a moving history of African American life and culture through the prism of Juneteenth. The award-winning Harvard historian presents an intimate portrait of the experiences of her family and her memories of life as an African American girl growing up in segregated Texas. The essays in her book invite readers to enter a world shaped by the forces of freedom and slavery.

Reed’s exploration of the history and legacy of Juneteenth is a poignant reminder of the hard history all Americans face.

‘O Freedom! Afro-American Emancipation Celebrations’

William H. Wiggins Jr.’s “ O Freedom! Afro-American Emancipation Celebrations ” is the historical standard for African American emancipation celebrations. It offers an accessible and well-researched account of the emergence and evolution of Juneteenth.

Wiggins brings together oral history with archival research to share the stories of how African Americans celebrated emancipation. It explains how Juneteenth is part of the tapestry of emancipation celebrations. These celebrations included such dates as January 1 , in North Carolina, April 3 , in Richmond, Virginia, and April 16 , in Washington, D.C.

What began as a local holiday has evolved into a national celebration.

Juneteenth celebrations are known for the variety of programs and events that highlight African American history and culture. In the 1960s, students at Prairie View A&M University in Prairie View, Texas, informed faculty that classes would not be held on Juneteenth. In Milwaukee, the local Juneteenth parade includes a group known as the Black Cowboys riding their horses along Dr. Martin Luther King Jr. Drive. Juneteenth celebrations also feature cultural fairs and exhibitions, artistic performances and historical reenactments. Lectures and public conversations, community feasts and religious services are also part of the celebrations.

‘Juneteenth’

Ralph Ellison, perhaps best known for his novel “Invisible Man,” offers multiple meanings of Juneteenth in African American and American life in his posthumously published novel “ Juneteenth .”

The ambivalence of Juneteenth is of a freedom delayed but not denied. Ellison’s spiraling novel captures this in the entangled and tragic lives of the racist Senator Sunraider – previously known as Bliss – and the minister who raised him, the Reverend A. Z. Hickman. For Ellison, Juneteenth represents more than just a celebration of emancipation. It also represents the shared fate of white Americans and African Americans in the quest to create a just and equal society. The promise and peril of Juneteenth is elegantly captured in Hickman’s words, “There’s been a heap of Juneteenths before this one and I tell you there’ll be a heap more before we’re truly free!”

‘Festivals of Freedom: Memory and Meaning in African American Emancipation Celebrations, 1808-1915’

Mitch Kachun’s book, “Festivals of Freedom: Memory and Meaning in African American Emancipation Celebrations, 1808-1915,” traces the history of emancipation celebrations and their influence on African American identity and community. Juneteenth joined a longer tradition of emancipation celebrations. Those celebrations included ones at the end of the trans-Atlantic slave trade in the United States on Jan. 1, 1808. They also included the August First Day/West India Day celebrations that marked the abolition of slavery throughout the British Empire on Aug. 1, 1834.

With an eye for historical detail, Kachun narrates a complex history of how Juneteenth and other freedom festivals shaped African American identity and political culture. The celebrations also displayed competing meanings of African American identity. In Washington, D.C. in the late 19th century, different groups of African Americans held distinct celebrations. These variations underscored tensions around political ideals, status and identity. Kachun’s book reminds us that Juneteenth served as a crucible for forging a collective and contested sense of African American community.

‘Rites of August First: Emancipation Day in the Black Atlantic World’

Similar to Kachun’s book, Howard University historian Jeffrey R. Kerr-Ritchie’s “ Rites of August First: Emancipation Day in the Black Atlantic World ” reminds readers of a broader history and geography of emancipation celebrations.

Kerr-Ritchie focuses on how various African American communities adopted and adapted West India Day celebrations. He also explores how they created meaning and culture in celebrating the abolition of slavery in the British West Indies. Kerr-Ritchie’s book details how these celebrations moved across political borders and boundaries.

‘Juneteenth: The Story Behind the Celebration’

Contemporary invocations of Juneteenth often overlook its military history.

Edward T. Cotham, Jr.’s “ Juneteenth: The Story Behind the Celebration ” fills the void by exploring the Civil War origins of Juneteenth.

Cotham renders explicit the military context leading up to the events on June 19, 1865, in Galveston. This is when enslaved Black people there finally got word that they had been freed more than two years prior. Cotham reminds readers that the history of Juneteenth involves ordinary actions of many individual people whose names may not be widely known.

Collectively, these books about Juneteenth offer fresh perspectives on the history and culture of African Americans on a quest to fully express their freedom. Juneteenth is also an invitation for all Americans to continue to learn about and strive for freedom for all people.

Corey D. B. Walker , Wake Forest Professor of the Humanities, Wake Forest University

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article . The   Conversation  is a nonprofit news source dedicated to spreading ideas and expertise from academia into  the  public discourse.  

More Articles

Research shows the history of black economic mobility, washington center for equitable growth, jun 19, 2024, how dei rollbacks at colleges and universities set back learning, the conversation, jun 11, 2024.

Become a newsletter subscriber to stay up-to-date on the latest Giving Compass news.

Donate to Giving Compass to help us guide donors toward practices that advance equity.

Partnerships

© 2024 Giving Compass Network

A 501(c)(3) organization. EIN: 85-1311683

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: 04 January 2010

The memory function of sleep

  • Susanne Diekelmann 1 &
  • Jan Born 1  

Nature Reviews Neuroscience volume  11 ,  pages 114–126 ( 2010 ) Cite this article

70k Accesses

2362 Citations

457 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Consolidation
  • Synaptic plasticity

Sleep promotes the consolidation of declarative as well as procedural and emotional memories in a wide variety of tasks. Sleep improves preferentially the consolidation of memories that were encoded explicitly and are behaviourally relevant to the individual.

Consolidation during sleep not only strengthens memory traces quantitatively but can also produce qualitative changes in memory representations. An active process of re-organization enables the formation of new associations and the extraction of generalized features. This can ease novel inferences and insights.

Spatio-temporal patterns of neuronal activity during encoding in the awake state become re-activated during subsequent sleep, specifically during slow-wave sleep (SWS) which is a state of minimum cholinergic activity. Such re-activations might promote the gradual redistribution of hippocampus-dependent memories from the hippocampus to neocortical sites for long-term storage (system consolidation) and might also trigger enduring synaptic changes to stabilize memories (synaptic consolidation).

Neocortical (<1 Hz) slow oscillations, thalamo-cortical spindles and hippocampal sharp-wave ripples are implicated in memory consolidation during SWS. The depolarizing up-states of the slow oscillations synchronously drive the generation of spindles and ripples accompanying hippocampal memory re-activations, thus providing a temporal frame for a fine-tuned hippocampus-to-neocortex transfer of memories.

Neocortical slow oscillations concurrently support a global synaptic downscaling that precludes saturation of synaptic networks and improves the capacity to encode new information.

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is characterized by a local upregulation of plasticity-related immediate early genes in the presence of high cholinergic activity and reduced electroencephalographic coherence between brain regions. These conditions might effectively support local synaptic consolidation.

The temporal sequence of SWS and REM sleep in the normal sleep cycle suggests that these sleep stages have complementary roles in memory consolidation: during SWS, system consolidation promotes the re-activation and redistribution of select memory traces for long-term storage, whereas ensuing REM sleep might act to stabilize the transformed memories by enabling undisturbed synaptic consolidation.

Sleep has been identified as a state that optimizes the consolidation of newly acquired information in memory, depending on the specific conditions of learning and the timing of sleep. Consolidation during sleep promotes both quantitative and qualitative changes of memory representations. Through specific patterns of neuromodulatory activity and electric field potential oscillations, slow-wave sleep (SWS) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep support system consolidation and synaptic consolidation, respectively. During SWS, slow oscillations, spindles and ripples — at minimum cholinergic activity — coordinate the re-activation and redistribution of hippocampus-dependent memories to neocortical sites, whereas during REM sleep, local increases in plasticity-related immediate-early gene activity — at high cholinergic and theta activity — might favour the subsequent synaptic consolidation of memories in the cortex.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 12 print issues and online access

176,64 € per year

only 14,72 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on Springer Link
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

learning and memory research paper

Similar content being viewed by others

learning and memory research paper

Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep

learning and memory research paper

Endogenous memory reactivation during sleep in humans is clocked by slow oscillation-spindle complexes

learning and memory research paper

Memory-enhancing properties of sleep depend on the oscillatory amplitude of norepinephrine

Siegel, J. M. Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 10 , 747–753 (2009).

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L. & O'Reilly, R. C. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102 , 419–457 (1995).

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Jenkins, J. G. & Dallenbach, K. M. Obliviscence during sleep and waking. Am. J. Psychol. 35 , 605–612 (1924).

Article   Google Scholar  

Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 437 , 1272–1278 (2005).

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Born, J., Rasch, B. & Gais, S. Sleep to remember. Neuroscientist 12 , 410–424 (2006).

Maquet, P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science 294 , 1048–1052 (2001).

Rasch, B. & Born, J. Maintaining memories by reactivation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17 , 698–703 (2007).

Marshall, L. & Born, J. The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 , 442–450 (2007).

Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A. & Miall, R. C. Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 5 , 576–582 (2004).

Smith, C. Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Med. Rev. 5 , 491–506 (2001).

Crick, F. & Mitchison, G. The function of dream sleep. Nature 304 , 111–114 (1983).

Barrett, T. R. & Ekstrand, B. R. Effect of sleep on memory. 3. Controlling for time-of-day effects. J. Exp. Psychol. 93 , 321–327 (1972).

Plihal, W. & Born, J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9 , 534–547 (1997). The first paper to show that SWS preferentially consolidates declarative memories, whereas REM sleep primarily supports procedural memories.

Tucker, M. A. et al. A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but not procedural memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 86 , 241–247 (2006).

Rasch, B., Buchel, C., Gais, S. & Born, J. Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science 315 , 1426–1429 (2007). Odours associated with the encoding of visuo-spatial memories were used as cues during post-learning SWS to re-activate the memories. The memory enhancement produced by this re-activation compellingly demonstrates a causal role of re-activations for sleep-dependent consolidation.

Lahl, O., Wispel, C., Willigens, B. & Pietrowsky, R. An ultra short episode of sleep is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance. J. Sleep Res. 17 , 3–10 (2008).

Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A. L. & Born, J. Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99 , 11987–11991 (2002).

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Walker, M. P. et al. Sleep and the time course of motor skill learning. Learn. Mem. 10 , 275–284 (2003).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Korman, M. et al. Daytime sleep condenses the time course of motor memory consolidation. Nature Neurosci. 10 , 1206–1213 (2007).

Gais, S., Molle, M., Helms, K. & Born, J. Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. J. Neurosci. 22 , 6830–6834 (2002).

Stickgold, R., Whidbee, D., Schirmer, B., Patel, V. & Hobson, J. A. Visual discrimination task improvement: A multi-step process occurring during sleep. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 , 246–254 (2000).

Stickgold, R., James, L. & Hobson, J. A. Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nature Neurosci. 3 , 1237–1238 (2000).

Mednick, S., Nakayama, K. & Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent learning: a nap is as good as a night. Nature Neurosci. 6 , 697–698 (2003).

Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A. & Stickgold, R. Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature 425 , 616–620 (2003).

Wagner, U., Gais, S. & Born, J. Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learn. Mem. 8 , 112–119 (2001).

Payne, J. D., Stickgold, R., Swanberg, K. & Kensinger, E. A. Sleep preferentially enhances memory for emotional components of scenes. Psychol. Sci. 19 , 781–788 (2008).

Nishida, M., Pearsall, J., Buckner, R. L. & Walker, M. P. REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cereb. Cortex 19 , 1158–1166 (2009).

Wagner, U., Hallschmid, M., Rasch, B. & Born, J. Brief sleep after learning keeps emotional memories alive for years. Biol. Psychiat. 60 , 788–790 (2006).

Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I. & Born, J. The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Med. Rev. 13 , 309–321 (2009).

Nishida, M. & Walker, M. P. Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally specific sleep spindles. PLoS ONE. 2 , e341 (2007).

Gais, S., Plihal, W., Wagner, U. & Born, J. Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nature Neurosci. 3 , 1335–1339 (2000).

Gais, S., Lucas, B. & Born, J. Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learn. Mem. 13 , 259–262 (2006).

Talamini, L. M., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., Takashima, A. & Jensen, O. Sleep directly following learning benefits consolidation of spatial associative memory. Learn. Mem. 15 , 233–237 (2008).

Robertson, E. M., Pascual-Leone, A. & Press, D. Z. Awareness modifies the skill-learning benefits of sleep. Curr. Biol. 14 , 208–212 (2004). By comparing effects of post-learning sleep on an implicitly and explicitly learned motor skill, this study showed that sleep preferentially benefits the consolidation of explicitly encoded memories.

Drosopoulos, S., Schulze, C., Fischer, S. & Born, J. Sleep's function in the spontaneous recovery and consolidation of memories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136 , 169–183 (2007).

Kuriyama, K., Stickgold, R. & Walker, M. P. Sleep-dependent learning and motor-skill complexity. Learn. Mem. 11 , 705–713 (2004).

Fischer, S. & Born, J. Anticipated reward enhances offline learning during sleep. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35 , 1586–1593 (2009).

Miller, E. K. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1 , 59–65 (2000).

Schendan, H. E., Searl, M. M., Melrose, R. J. & Stern, C. E. An FMRI study of the role of the medial temporal lobe in implicit and explicit sequence learning. Neuron 37 , 1013–1025 (2003).

Wagner, A. D. et al. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281 , 1188–1191 (1998).

Ellenbogen, J. M., Hulbert, J. C., Stickgold, R., Dinges, D. F. & Thompson-Schill, S. L. Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: sleep, declarative memory, and associative interference. Curr. Biol. 16 , 1290–1294 (2006).

Hotermans, C., Peigneux, P., Maertens de, N. A., Moonen, G. & Maquet, P. Early boost and slow consolidation in motor skill learning. Learn. Mem. 13 , 580–583 (2006).

Rickard, T. C., Cai, D. J., Rieth, C. A., Jones, J. & Ard, M. C. Sleep does not enhance motor sequence learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34 , 834–842 (2008).

Wixted, J. T. The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55 , 235–269 (2004).

Ellenbogen, J. M., Payne, J. D. & Stickgold, R. The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16 , 716–722 (2006).

Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R. & Born, J. Sleep inspires insight. Nature 427 , 352–355 (2004). An experimental demonstration that sleep promotes insight into a logical problem, which can be considered a behavioural proof that memory representations undergo qualitative changes during sleep.

Ellenbogen, J. M., Hu, P. T., Payne, J. D., Titone, D. & Walker, M. P. Human relational memory requires time and sleep. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104 , 7723–7728 (2007).

Fischer, S., Drosopoulos, S., Tsen, J. & Born, J. Implicit learning -- explicit knowing: a role for sleep in memory system interaction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18 , 311–319 (2006).

Robertson, E. M. From creation to consolidation: a novel framework for memory processing. PLoS Biol. 7 , e19 (2009).

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   CAS   Google Scholar  

Brown, R. M. & Robertson, E. M. Off-line processing: reciprocal interactions between declarative and procedural memories. J. Neurosci. 27 , 10468–10475 (2007).

Born, J. & Gais, S. REM sleep deprivation: the wrong paradigm leading to wrong conclusions. Behav. Brain Sci. 23 , 912–913 (2000).

Horne, J. A. & McGrath, M. J. The consolidation hypothesis for REM sleep function: stress and other confounding factors — a review. Biol. Psychol. 18 , 165–184 (1984).

Hennevin, E., Hars, B., Maho, C. & Bloch, V. Processing of learned information in paradoxical sleep: relevance for memory. Behav. Brain Res. 69 , 125–135 (1995).

Smith, C. The REM sleep window and memory processing in Sleep and brain plasticity (eds. Maquet, P., Smith, C. & Stickgold, R.) 117–133 (Oxford University Press, New York, 2003).

Book   Google Scholar  

Smith, C. Sleep states and memory processes. Behav. Brain Res. 69 , 137–145 (1995).

Rasch, B., Pommer, J., Diekelmann, S. & Born, J. Pharmacological REM sleep suppression paradoxically improves rather than impairs skill memory. Nature Neurosci. 12 , 396–397 (2009).

Vertes, R. P. & Siegel, J. M. Time for the sleep community to take a critical look at the purported role of sleep in memory processing. Sleep 28 , 1228–1229 (2005).

Calabrese, F., Molteni, R., Racagni, G. & Riva, M. A. Neuronal plasticity: A link between stress and mood disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2009).

Plihal, W. & Born, J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology 36 , 571–582 (1999).

Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M. & Tononi, G. Local sleep and learning. Nature 430 , 78–81 (2004). Using high-density EEG in humans the experiments reveal a local increase in slow wave activity (SWA) over motor cortical areas during sleep after learning a motor skill, which was correlated with the sleep-induced gain in skill. The experiments show that the homeostatic regulation of SWA is locally influenced by prior learning and suggest that this activity contributes to consolidation.

Aeschbach, D., Cutler, A. J. & Ronda, J. M. A role for non-rapid-eye-movement sleep homeostasis in perceptual learning. J. Neurosci. 28 , 2766–2772 (2008).

Fogel, S. M., Smith, C. T. & Cote, K. A. Dissociable learning-dependent changes in REM and non-REM sleep in declarative and procedural memory systems. Behav. Brain Res. 180 , 48–61 (2007).

Rauchs, G. et al. Consolidation of strictly episodic memories mainly requires rapid eye movement sleep. Sleep 27 , 395–401 (2004).

Giuditta, A. et al. The sequential hypothesis of the function of sleep. Behav. Brain Res. 69 , 157–166 (1995).

Ficca, G. & Salzarulo, P. What in sleep is for memory. Sleep Med. 5 , 225–230 (2004).

Nader, R. & Smith, C. A role for stage 2 sleep in memory processing in Sleep and brain plasticity (eds. Maquet, P., Smith, C. & Stickgold, R.) 87–98 (Oxford University Press, New York, 2003).

Datta, S. Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J. Neurosci. 20 , 8607–8613 (2000).

Hebb, D. O. The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory . (John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1949).

Google Scholar  

Pavlides, C. & Winson, J. Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the awake state on the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes. J. Neurosci. 9 , 2907–2918 (1989).

Wilson, M. A. & McNaughton, B. L. Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265 , 676–679 (1994). A pioneering study revealing that in rats spatial–temporal patterns of neuronal firing in the hippocampus during learning are re-activated in the same order during subsequent SWS.

Nadasdy, Z., Hirase, H., Czurko, A., Csicsvari, J. & Buzsaki, G. Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 19 , 9497–9507 (1999).

Ji, D. & Wilson, M. A. Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nature Neurosci. 10 , 100–107 (2007). The first study to report that neuronal ensembles in the hippocampus and neocortex become re-activated in parallel during SWS in temporal frames corresponding to the slow oscillation.

Euston, D. R., Tatsuno, M. & McNaughton, B. L. Fast-forward playback of recent memory sequences in prefrontal cortex during sleep. Science 318 , 1147–1150 (2007).

Lansink, C. S. et al. Preferential reactivation of motivationally relevant information in the ventral striatum. J. Neurosci. 28 , 6372–6382 (2008).

Tatsuno, M., Lipa, P. & McNaughton, B. L. Methodological considerations on the use of template matching to study long-lasting memory trace replay. J. Neurosci. 26 , 10727–10742 (2006).

Louie, K. & Wilson, M. A. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron 29 , 145–156 (2001).

Poe, G. R., Nitz, D. A., McNaughton, B. L. & Barnes, C. A. Experience-dependent phase-reversal of hippocampal neuron firing during REM sleep. Brain Res. 855 , 176–180 (2000).

Ribeiro, S. et al. Long-lasting novelty-induced neuronal reverberation during slow-wave sleep in multiple forebrain areas. PLoS Biol. 2 , e24 (2004).

Foster, D. J. & Wilson, M. A. Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature 440 , 680–683 (2006).

Peigneux, P. et al. Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron 44 , 535–545 (2004).

Maquet, P. et al. Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep. Nature Neurosci. 3 , 831–836 (2000).

Gais, S. et al. Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104 , 18778–18783 (2007). Using functional brain imaging the authors show that sleep leads to a redistribution of memory traces from the hippocampus to neocortical sites for long-term storage.

Takashima, A. et al. Declarative memory consolidation in humans: a prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103 , 756–761 (2006).

Dudai, Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55 , 51–86 (2004).

Frankland, P. W. & Bontempi, B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 6 , 119–130 (2005).

Bramham, C. R. & Srebro, B. Synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus is modulated by behavioral state. Brain Res. 493 , 74–86 (1989).

Rosanova, M. & Ulrich, D. Pattern-specific associative long-term potentiation induced by a sleep spindle-related spike train. J. Neurosci. 25 , 9398–9405 (2005).

King, C., Henze, D. A., Leinekugel, X. & Buzsaki, G. Hebbian modification of a hippocampal population pattern in the rat. J. Physiol. 521 , 159–167 (1999).

Czarnecki, A., Birtoli, B. & Ulrich, D. Cellular mechanisms of burst firing-mediated long-term depression in rat neocortical pyramidal cells. J. Physiol. 578 , 471–479 (2007).

Romcy-Pereira, R. & Pavlides, C. Distinct modulatory effects of sleep on the maintenance of hippocampal and medial prefrontal cortex LTP. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20 , 3453–3462 (2004).

Bergmann, T. O. et al. A local signature of LTP- and LTD-like plasticity in human NREM sleep. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27 , 2241–2249 (2008).

Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., Pfister-Genskow, M., Faraguna, U. & Tononi, G. Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nature Neurosci. 11 , 200–208 (2008). The authors demonstrate that physiological markers of synaptic strength increase during waking and decrease during sleep. The results provide strong evidence for global synaptic downscaling during sleep.

Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Differential expression of plasticity-related genes in waking and sleep and their regulation by the noradrenergic system. J. Neurosci. 20 , 9187–9194 (2000).

Dash, M. B., Douglas, C. L., Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C. & Tononi, G. Long-term homeostasis of extracellular glutamate in the rat cerebral cortex across sleep and waking states. J. Neurosci. 29 , 620–629 (2009).

Cirelli, C., Gutierrez, C. M. & Tononi, G. Extensive and divergent effects of sleep and wakefulness on brain gene expression. Neuron 41 , 35–43 (2004).

Ribeiro, S., Goyal, V., Mello, C. V. & Pavlides, C. Brain gene expression during REM sleep depends on prior waking experience. Learn. Mem. 6 , 500–508 (1999).

Ribeiro, S. et al. Induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation during waking leads to increased extrahippocampal zif-268 expression during ensuing rapid-eye-movement sleep. J. Neurosci. 22 , 10914–10923 (2002). An important study showing that hippocampal activity during waking produces an increase in plasticity-related IEG expression in specific cortical areas during subsequent REM sleep, supporting a role of REM sleep in synaptic consolidation.

Ribeiro, S. et al. Novel experience induces persistent sleep-dependent plasticity in the cortex but not in the hippocampus. Front. Neurosci. 1 , 43–55 (2007).

Frank, M. G., Jha, S. K. & Coleman, T. Blockade of postsynaptic activity in sleep inhibits developmental plasticity in visual cortex. Neuroreport 17 , 1459–1463 (2006).

Frank, M. G., Issa, N. P. & Stryker, M. P. Sleep enhances plasticity in the developing visual cortex. Neuron 30 , 275–287 (2001).

Aton, S. J. et al. Mechanisms of sleep-dependent consolidation of cortical plasticity. Neuron 61 , 454–466 (2009).

Gais, S., Rasch, B., Wagner, U. & Born, J. Visual-procedural memory consolidation during sleep blocked by glutamatergic receptor antagonists. J. Neurosci. 28 , 5513–5518 (2008).

Buzsaki, G. & Draguhn, A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304 , 1926–1929 (2004).

Buzsaki, G. Rhythms of the brain (Oxford University Press, New York, 2006).

Sirota, A., Csicsvari, J., Buhl, D. & Buzsaki, G. Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100 , 2065–2069 (2003). First study in rats and mice that demonstrated a temporally fine-tuned relationship between slow oscillations, spindles and sharp-wave ripples possibly underlying the transfer of information between the hippocampus and neocortical regions.

Sirota, A. & Buzsaki, G. Interaction between neocortical and hippocampal networks via slow oscillations. Thalamus. Relat Syst. 3 , 245–259 (2005).

Molle, M., Marshall, L., Gais, S. & Born, J. Learning increases human electroencephalographic coherence during subsequent slow sleep oscillations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101 , 13963–13968 (2004).

Molle, M., Eschenko, O., Gais, S., Sara, S. J. & Born, J. The influence of learning on sleep slow oscillations and associated spindles and ripples in humans and rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29 , 1071–1081 (2009).

Huber, R. et al. Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nature Neurosci. 9 , 1169–1176 (2006).

Huber, R. et al. TMS-induced cortical potentiation during wakefulness locally increases slow wave activity during sleep. PLoS ONE. 2 , e276 (2007).

Huber, R. et al. Measures of cortical plasticity after transcranial paired associative stimulation predict changes in electroencephalogram slow-wave activity during subsequent sleep. J. Neurosci. 28 , 7911–7918 (2008).

Marshall, L., Helgadottir, H., Molle, M. & Born, J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444 , 610–613 (2006). By applying electrical stimulation (at the slow oscillation frequency) to healthy humans the authors induced slow oscillation activity during non-REM sleep and improved the retention of memories in humans. These findings provide first evidence for a causal contribution of slow oscillations to sleep-dependent memory consolidation.

Werk, C. M., Harbour, V. L. & Chapman, C. A. Induction of long-term potentiation leads to increased reliability of evoked neocortical spindles in vivo . Neuroscience 131 , 793–800 (2005).

Schabus, M. et al. Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep 27 , 1479–1485 (2004).

Fogel, S. M. & Smith, C. T. Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 sleep. J. Sleep Res. 15 , 250–255 (2006).

Eschenko, O., Molle, M., Born, J. & Sara, S. J. Elevated sleep spindle density after learning or after retrieval in rats. J. Neurosci. 26 , 12914–12920 (2006).

Schmidt, C. et al. Encoding difficulty promotes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. J. Neurosci. 26 , 8976–8982 (2006).

Morin, A. et al. Motor sequence learning increases sleep spindles and fast frequencies in post-training sleep. Sleep 31 , 1149–1156 (2008).

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Clemens, Z., Fabo, D. & Halasz, P. Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience 132 , 529–535 (2005).

Clemens, Z., Fabo, D. & Halasz, P. Twenty-four hours retention of visuospatial memory correlates with the number of parietal sleep spindles. Neurosci. Lett. 403 , 52–56 (2006).

Buzsaki, G. Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for “noisy” brain states. Neuroscience 31 , 551–570 (1989).

Peyrache, A., Khamassi, M., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I. & Battaglia, F. P. Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nature Neurosci. 12 , 919–926 (2009).

Behrens, C. J., van den Boom, L. P., de, H. L., Friedman, A. & Heinemann, U. Induction of sharp wave-ripple complexes in vitro and reorganization of hippocampal networks. Nature Neurosci. 8 , 1560–1567 (2005).

Buzsaki, G., Haas, H. L. & Anderson, E. G. Long-term potentiation induced by physiologically relevant stimulus patterns. Brain Res. 435 , 331–333 (1987).

Csicsvari, J., Hirase, H., Mamiya, A. & Buzsaki, G. Ensemble patterns of hippocampal CA3-CA1 neurons during sharp wave-associated population events. Neuron 28 , 585–594 (2000).

Eschenko, O., Ramadan, W., Molle, M., Born, J. & Sara, S. J. Sustained increase in hippocampal sharp-wave ripple activity during slow-wave sleep after learning. Learn. Mem. 15 , 222–228 (2008).

Axmacher, N., Elger, C. E. & Fell, J. Ripples in the medial temporal lobe are relevant for human memory consolidation. Brain 131 , 1806–1817 (2008).

Girardeau, G., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I., Buzsaki, G. & Zugaro, M. B. Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nature Neurosci. 12 , 1222–1223 (2009). The authors showed that the suppression of hippocampal ripples by electrical pulses during post-learning rest in rats impaired the consolidation of a hippocampus-dependent spatial task. These experiments provide direct evidence for an involvement of hippocampal sharp-wave ripples in the off-line consolidation of memory.

Isomura, Y. et al. Integration and segregation of activity in entorhinal-hippocampal subregions by neocortical slow oscillations. Neuron 52 , 871–882 (2006).

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Molle, M., Marshall, L., Gais, S. & Born, J. Grouping of spindle activity during slow oscillations in human non-rapid eye movement sleep. J. Neurosci. 22 , 10941–10947 (2002).

Molle, M., Yeshenko, O., Marshall, L., Sara, S. J. & Born, J. Hippocampal sharp wave-ripples linked to slow oscillations in rat slow-wave sleep. J. Neurophysiol. 96 , 62–70 (2006).

Steriade, M. Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 137 , 1087–1106 (2006).

Lestienne, R., Herve-Minvielle, A., Robinson, D., Briois, L. & Sara, S. J. Slow oscillations as a probe of the dynamics of the locus coeruleus-frontal cortex interaction in anesthetized rats. J. Physiol. Paris 91 , 273–284 (1997).

Eschenko, O. & Sara, S. J. Learning-dependent, transient increase of activity in noradrenergic neurons of locus coeruleus during slow wave sleep in the rat: brain stem-cortex interplay for memory consolidation? Cereb. Cortex 18 , 2596–2603 (2008).

Siapas, A. G. & Wilson, M. A. Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21 , 1123–1128 (1998).

Wierzynski, C. M., Lubenov, E. V., Gu, M. & Siapas, A. G. State-dependent spike-timing relationships between hippocampal and prefrontal circuits during sleep. Neuron 61 , 587–596 (2009).

Buzsaki, G. Memory consolidation during sleep: a neurophysiological perspective. J. Sleep Res. 7 (Suppl 1), 17–23 (1998).

Molle, M. & Born, J. Hippocampus whispering in deep sleep to prefrontal cortex — for good memories? Neuron 61 , 496–498 (2009).

Article   PubMed   CAS   Google Scholar  

Ulloor, J. & Datta, S. Spatio-temporal activation of cyclic AMP response element-binding protein, activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein and brain-derived nerve growth factor: a mechanism for pontine-wave generator activation-dependent two-way active-avoidance memory processing in the rat. J. Neurochem. 95 , 418–428 (2005).

Datta, S., Li, G. & Auerbach, S. Activation of phasic pontine-wave generator in the rat: a mechanism for expression of plasticity-related genes and proteins in the dorsal hippocampus and amygdala. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27 , 1876–1892 (2008).

Buzsaki, G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33 , 325–340 (2002).

Holscher, C., Anwyl, R. & Rowan, M. J. Stimulation on the positive phase of hippocampal theta rhythm induces long-term potentiation that can be depotentiated by stimulation on the negative phase in area CA1 in vivo . J. Neurosci. 17 , 6470–6477 (1997).

Hellman, K. & Abel, T. Fear conditioning increases NREM sleep. Behav. Neurosci. 121 , 310–323 (2007).

Axmacher, N., Helmstaedter, C., Elger, C. E. & Fell, J. Enhancement of neocortical-medial temporal EEG correlations during non-REM sleep. Neural Plast. 2008 , e563028 (2008).

Cantero, J. L. et al. Sleep-dependent theta oscillations in the human hippocampus and neocortex. J. Neurosci. 23 , 10897–10903 (2003).

Montgomery, S. M., Sirota, A. & Buzsaki, G. Theta and gamma coordination of hippocampal networks during waking and rapid eye movement sleep. J. Neurosci. 28 , 6731–6741 (2008).

Tononi, G. & Cirelli, C. Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 10 , 49–62 (2006).

Kemp, N. & Bashir, Z. I. Long-term depression: a cascade of induction and expression mechanisms. Prog. Neurobiol. 65 , 339–365 (2001).

Fischer, S., Nitschke, M. F., Melchert, U. H., Erdmann, C. & Born, J. Motor memory consolidation in sleep shapes more effective neuronal representations. J. Neurosci. 25 , 11248–11255 (2005).

Orban, P. et al. Sleep after spatial learning promotes covert reorganization of brain activity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103 , 7124–7129 (2006).

Van Der Werf, Y. D. et al. Sleep benefits subsequent hippocampal functioning. Nature Neurosci. 12 , 122–123 (2009).

Marr, D. Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 262 , 23–81 (1971).

Gais, S. & Born, J. Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for declarative memory consolidation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101 , 2140–2144 (2004).

Hasselmo, M. E. Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and memory consolidation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3 , 351–359 (1999).

Hasselmo, M. E. & McGaughy, J. High acetylcholine levels set circuit dynamics for attention and encoding and low acetylcholine levels set dynamics for consolidation. Prog. Brain Res. 145 , 207–231 (2004).

Lansink, C. S., Goltstein, P. M., Lankelma, J. V., McNaughton, B. L. & Pennartz, C. M. Hippocampus leads ventral striatum in replay of place-reward information. PLoS Biol. 7 , e1000173 (2009).

Sejnowski, T. J. & Destexhe, A. Why do we sleep? Brain Res. 886 , 208–223 (2000).

Sterpenich, V. et al. Sleep promotes the neural reorganization of remote emotional memory. J. Neurosci. 29 , 5143–5152 (2009).

Murphy, M. et al. Source modeling sleep slow waves. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106 , 1608–1613 (2009).

Massimini, M., Huber, R., Ferrarelli, F., Hill, S. & Tononi, G. The sleep slow oscillation as a traveling wave. J. Neurosci. 24 , 6862–6870 (2004).

von der Kammer, H. et al. Muscarinic acetylcholine receptors activate expression of the EGR gene family of transcription factors. J. Biol. Chem. 273 , 14538–14544 (1998).

Teber, I., Kohling, R., Speckmann, E. J., Barnekow, A. & Kremerskothen, J. Muscarinic acetylcholine receptor stimulation induces expression of the activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated gene (ARC). Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 121 , 131–136 (2004).

Lopes Aguiar, C. et al. Muscarinic acetylcholine neurotransmission enhances the late-phase of long-term potentiation in the hippocampal-prefrontal cortex pathway of rats in vivo : a possible involvement of monoaminergic systems. Neuroscience 153 , 1309–1319 (2008).

Achermann, P. & Borbely, A. A. Low-frequency (&lt; 1 Hz) oscillations in the human sleep electroencephalogram. Neuroscience 81 , 213–222 (1997).

Destexhe, A., Hughes, S. W., Rudolph, M. & Crunelli, V. Are corticothalamic 'up' states fragments of wakefulness? Trends Neurosci. 30 , 334–342 (2007).

Luczak, A., Bartho, P., Marguet, S. L., Buzsaki, G. & Harris, K. D. Sequential structure of neocortical spontaneous activity in vivo . Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104 , 347–352 (2007).

Bazhenov, M., Timofeev, I., Steriade, M. & Sejnowski, T. J. Model of thalamocortical slow-wave sleep oscillations and transitions to activated states. J. Neurosci. 22 , 8691–8704 (2002).

Timofeev, I. & Bazhenov, M. Mechanisms and biological role of thalamocortical oscillations in Trends in Chronobiology Research (ed. Columbus, F.) 1–47 (Nova Science Publishers, Inc, 2005).

DeGennaro, L. & Ferrara, M. Sleep spindles: an overview. Sleep Med. Rev. 7 , 423–440 (2003).

Karlsson, M. P. & Frank, L. M. Awake replay of remote experiences in the hippocampus. Nature Neurosci. 12 , 913–918 (2009).

Pace-Schott, E. F. & Hobson, J. A. The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3 , 591–605 (2002).

Rasch, B. H., Born, J. & Gais, S. Combined blockade of cholinergic receptors shifts the brain from stimulus encoding to memory consolidation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18 , 793–802 (2006).

de Kloet, E. R., Vreugdenhil, E., Oitzl, M. S. & Joels, M. Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocr. Rev. 19 , 269–301 (1998).

Wagner, U. & Born, J. Memory consolidation during sleep: interactive effects of sleep stages and HPA regulation. Stress 11 , 28–41 (2008).

Legault, G., Smith, C. T. & Beninger, R. J. Post-training intra-striatal scopolamine or flupenthixol impairs radial maze learning in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 170 , 148–155 (2006).

Rasch, B., Gais, S. & Born, J. Impaired off-line consolidation of motor memories after combined blockade of cholinergic receptors during REM sleep-rich sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology 34 , 1843–1853 (2009).

Hornung, O. P., Regen, F., Danker-Hopfe, H., Schredl, M. & Heuser, I. The relationship between REM sleep and memory consolidation in old age and effects of cholinergic medication. Biol. Psychiatry 61 , 750–757 (2007).

Walker, M. P. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1156 , 168–197 (2009).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We apologize to those whose work was not cited because of space constraints. We thank Drs. B. Rasch, L. Marshall, I. Wilhelm, M. Hallschmid, E. Robertson and S. Ribeiro for helpful discussions and comments on earlier drafts. This work was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB 654 'Plasticity and Sleep').

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Neuroendocrinology, University of Lübeck, Haus 50, 2. OG, Ratzeburger Allee 160, Lübeck, 23538, Germany

Susanne Diekelmann & Jan Born

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan Born .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Related links

Further information.

Jan Born's homepage

Memories that are accessible to conscious recollection including memories for facts and episodes, for example, learning vocabulary or remembering events. Declarative memories rely on the hippocampus and associated medial temporal lobe structures, together with neocortical regions for long-term storage.

Memories for skills that result from repeated practice and are not necessarily available for conscious recollection, for example, riding a bike or playing the piano. Procedural memories rely on the striatum and cerebellum, although recent studies indicate that the hippocampus can also be implicated in procedural learning.

A task in which subjects are required to rapidly respond to different spatial cues by pressing corresponding buttons. This task can be performed implicitly (that is, without knowledge that there is a regularity underlying the sequence of cue positions) or explicitly (by informing the subject about this underlying regularity).

Learning without being aware that something is being learned.

Learning while being aware that something is being learned.

Different types of memory, such as declarative and non-declarative memory, are thought to be mediated by distinct neural systems, the organization of which is still a topic of debate.

Short transitory periods of sleep in rats that, based on EEG criteria, can neither be classified as REM sleep or SWS.

Genes that encode transcription factors that are induced within minutes of raised neuronal activity without requiring a protein signal. Immediate-early gene activation is, therefore, used as an indirect marker of neuronal activation. The immediate early genes Arc and Egr1 ( zif268 ) are associated with synaptic plasticity.

Refers to the functional changes at synapses that increase the efficacy of synaptic transmission and occurs when the presynaptic neuron repeatedly and persistently stimulates the postsynaptic neuron.

Refers to the functional changes at synapses that alter the efficacy of synaptic transmission depending on the relative timing of pre- and postsynaptic firing ('spiking'). The synaptic connection is strengthened if the presynaptic neuron fires shortly before the postsynaptic neuron, but is weakened if the sequence of firing is reversed.

The slow oscillations that predominate EEG activity during SWS are characterized by alternating states of neuronal silence with an absence of spiking activity and membrane hyperpolarization in all cortical neurons ('down-state') and strongly increased wake-like firing of large neuronal populations and membrane depolarization ('up-state').

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Diekelmann, S., Born, J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 11 , 114–126 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762

Download citation

Published : 04 January 2010

Issue Date : February 2010

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

The cognitive impact of light: illuminating iprgc circuit mechanisms.

  • Heather L. Mahoney
  • Tiffany M. Schmidt

Nature Reviews Neuroscience (2024)

An update on recent advances in targeted memory reactivation during sleep

  • Julia Carbone
  • Susanne Diekelmann

npj Science of Learning (2024)

Altered sleep behavior strengthens face validity in the ArcAβ mouse model for Alzheimer’s disease

  • Alp Altunkaya
  • Cassandra Deichsel
  • Thomas Fenzl

Scientific Reports (2024)

Carbon supported ternary layered double hydroxide nanocomposite for Fluoxetine removal and subsequent utilization of spent adsorbent as antidepressant

  • Samar M. Mahgoub
  • Rehab Mahmoud

No difference between using short and long intervals for distributed proficiency-based laparoscopy simulator training: a randomized trial

  • Diana Hai Yen Tang
  • Theresa Bruun Østdal
  • Flemming Bjerrum

Surgical Endoscopy (2024)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

learning and memory research paper

The state of AI in 2023: Generative AI’s breakout year

You have reached a page with older survey data. please see our 2024 survey results here ..

The latest annual McKinsey Global Survey  on the current state of AI confirms the explosive growth of generative AI (gen AI) tools . Less than a year after many of these tools debuted, one-third of our survey respondents say their organizations are using gen AI regularly in at least one business function. Amid recent advances, AI has risen from a topic relegated to tech employees to a focus of company leaders: nearly one-quarter of surveyed C-suite executives say they are personally using gen AI tools for work, and more than one-quarter of respondents from companies using AI say gen AI is already on their boards’ agendas. What’s more, 40 percent of respondents say their organizations will increase their investment in AI overall because of advances in gen AI. The findings show that these are still early days for managing gen AI–related risks, with less than half of respondents saying their organizations are mitigating even the risk they consider most relevant: inaccuracy.

The organizations that have already embedded AI capabilities have been the first to explore gen AI’s potential, and those seeing the most value from more traditional AI capabilities—a group we call AI high performers—are already outpacing others in their adoption of gen AI tools. 1 We define AI high performers as organizations that, according to respondents, attribute at least 20 percent of their EBIT to AI adoption.

The expected business disruption from gen AI is significant, and respondents predict meaningful changes to their workforces. They anticipate workforce cuts in certain areas and large reskilling efforts to address shifting talent needs. Yet while the use of gen AI might spur the adoption of other AI tools, we see few meaningful increases in organizations’ adoption of these technologies. The percent of organizations adopting any AI tools has held steady since 2022, and adoption remains concentrated within a small number of business functions.

Table of Contents

  • It’s early days still, but use of gen AI is already widespread
  • Leading companies are already ahead with gen AI
  • AI-related talent needs shift, and AI’s workforce effects are expected to be substantial
  • With all eyes on gen AI, AI adoption and impact remain steady

About the research

1. it’s early days still, but use of gen ai is already widespread.

The findings from the survey—which was in the field in mid-April 2023—show that, despite gen AI’s nascent public availability, experimentation with the tools  is already relatively common, and respondents expect the new capabilities to transform their industries. Gen AI has captured interest across the business population: individuals across regions, industries, and seniority levels are using gen AI for work and outside of work. Seventy-nine percent of all respondents say they’ve had at least some exposure to gen AI, either for work or outside of work, and 22 percent say they are regularly using it in their own work. While reported use is quite similar across seniority levels, it is highest among respondents working in the technology sector and those in North America.

Organizations, too, are now commonly using gen AI. One-third of all respondents say their organizations are already regularly using generative AI in at least one function—meaning that 60 percent of organizations with reported AI adoption are using gen AI. What’s more, 40 percent of those reporting AI adoption at their organizations say their companies expect to invest more in AI overall thanks to generative AI, and 28 percent say generative AI use is already on their board’s agenda. The most commonly reported business functions using these newer tools are the same as those in which AI use is most common overall: marketing and sales, product and service development, and service operations, such as customer care and back-office support. This suggests that organizations are pursuing these new tools where the most value is. In our previous research , these three areas, along with software engineering, showed the potential to deliver about 75 percent of the total annual value from generative AI use cases.

In these early days, expectations for gen AI’s impact are high : three-quarters of all respondents expect gen AI to cause significant or disruptive change in the nature of their industry’s competition in the next three years. Survey respondents working in the technology and financial-services industries are the most likely to expect disruptive change from gen AI. Our previous research shows  that, while all industries are indeed likely to see some degree of disruption, the level of impact is likely to vary. 2 “ The economic potential of generative AI: The next productivity frontier ,” McKinsey, June 14, 2023. Industries relying most heavily on knowledge work are likely to see more disruption—and potentially reap more value. While our estimates suggest that tech companies, unsurprisingly, are poised to see the highest impact from gen AI—adding value equivalent to as much as 9 percent of global industry revenue—knowledge-based industries such as banking (up to 5 percent), pharmaceuticals and medical products (also up to 5 percent), and education (up to 4 percent) could experience significant effects as well. By contrast, manufacturing-based industries, such as aerospace, automotives, and advanced electronics, could experience less disruptive effects. This stands in contrast to the impact of previous technology waves that affected manufacturing the most and is due to gen AI’s strengths in language-based activities, as opposed to those requiring physical labor.

Responses show many organizations not yet addressing potential risks from gen AI

According to the survey, few companies seem fully prepared for the widespread use of gen AI—or the business risks these tools may bring. Just 21 percent of respondents reporting AI adoption say their organizations have established policies governing employees’ use of gen AI technologies in their work. And when we asked specifically about the risks of adopting gen AI, few respondents say their companies are mitigating the most commonly cited risk with gen AI: inaccuracy. Respondents cite inaccuracy more frequently than both cybersecurity and regulatory compliance, which were the most common risks from AI overall in previous surveys. Just 32 percent say they’re mitigating inaccuracy, a smaller percentage than the 38 percent who say they mitigate cybersecurity risks. Interestingly, this figure is significantly lower than the percentage of respondents who reported mitigating AI-related cybersecurity last year (51 percent). Overall, much as we’ve seen in previous years, most respondents say their organizations are not addressing AI-related risks.

2. Leading companies are already ahead with gen AI

The survey results show that AI high performers—that is, organizations where respondents say at least 20 percent of EBIT in 2022 was attributable to AI use—are going all in on artificial intelligence, both with gen AI and more traditional AI capabilities. These organizations that achieve significant value from AI are already using gen AI in more business functions than other organizations do, especially in product and service development and risk and supply chain management. When looking at all AI capabilities—including more traditional machine learning capabilities, robotic process automation, and chatbots—AI high performers also are much more likely than others to use AI in product and service development, for uses such as product-development-cycle optimization, adding new features to existing products, and creating new AI-based products. These organizations also are using AI more often than other organizations in risk modeling and for uses within HR such as performance management and organization design and workforce deployment optimization.

AI high performers are much more likely than others to use AI in product and service development.

Another difference from their peers: high performers’ gen AI efforts are less oriented toward cost reduction, which is a top priority at other organizations. Respondents from AI high performers are twice as likely as others to say their organizations’ top objective for gen AI is to create entirely new businesses or sources of revenue—and they’re most likely to cite the increase in the value of existing offerings through new AI-based features.

As we’ve seen in previous years , these high-performing organizations invest much more than others in AI: respondents from AI high performers are more than five times more likely than others to say they spend more than 20 percent of their digital budgets on AI. They also use AI capabilities more broadly throughout the organization. Respondents from high performers are much more likely than others to say that their organizations have adopted AI in four or more business functions and that they have embedded a higher number of AI capabilities. For example, respondents from high performers more often report embedding knowledge graphs in at least one product or business function process, in addition to gen AI and related natural-language capabilities.

While AI high performers are not immune to the challenges of capturing value from AI, the results suggest that the difficulties they face reflect their relative AI maturity, while others struggle with the more foundational, strategic elements of AI adoption. Respondents at AI high performers most often point to models and tools, such as monitoring model performance in production and retraining models as needed over time, as their top challenge. By comparison, other respondents cite strategy issues, such as setting a clearly defined AI vision that is linked with business value or finding sufficient resources.

The findings offer further evidence that even high performers haven’t mastered best practices regarding AI adoption, such as machine-learning-operations (MLOps) approaches, though they are much more likely than others to do so. For example, just 35 percent of respondents at AI high performers report that where possible, their organizations assemble existing components, rather than reinvent them, but that’s a much larger share than the 19 percent of respondents from other organizations who report that practice.

Many specialized MLOps technologies and practices  may be needed to adopt some of the more transformative uses cases that gen AI applications can deliver—and do so as safely as possible. Live-model operations is one such area, where monitoring systems and setting up instant alerts to enable rapid issue resolution can keep gen AI systems in check. High performers stand out in this respect but have room to grow: one-quarter of respondents from these organizations say their entire system is monitored and equipped with instant alerts, compared with just 12 percent of other respondents.

3. AI-related talent needs shift, and AI’s workforce effects are expected to be substantial

Our latest survey results show changes in the roles that organizations are filling to support their AI ambitions. In the past year, organizations using AI most often hired data engineers, machine learning engineers, and Al data scientists—all roles that respondents commonly reported hiring in the previous survey. But a much smaller share of respondents report hiring AI-related-software engineers—the most-hired role last year—than in the previous survey (28 percent in the latest survey, down from 39 percent). Roles in prompt engineering have recently emerged, as the need for that skill set rises alongside gen AI adoption, with 7 percent of respondents whose organizations have adopted AI reporting those hires in the past year.

The findings suggest that hiring for AI-related roles remains a challenge but has become somewhat easier over the past year, which could reflect the spate of layoffs at technology companies from late 2022 through the first half of 2023. Smaller shares of respondents than in the previous survey report difficulty hiring for roles such as AI data scientists, data engineers, and data-visualization specialists, though responses suggest that hiring machine learning engineers and AI product owners remains as much of a challenge as in the previous year.

Looking ahead to the next three years, respondents predict that the adoption of AI will reshape many roles in the workforce. Generally, they expect more employees to be reskilled than to be separated. Nearly four in ten respondents reporting AI adoption expect more than 20 percent of their companies’ workforces will be reskilled, whereas 8 percent of respondents say the size of their workforces will decrease by more than 20 percent.

Looking specifically at gen AI’s predicted impact, service operations is the only function in which most respondents expect to see a decrease in workforce size at their organizations. This finding generally aligns with what our recent research  suggests: while the emergence of gen AI increased our estimate of the percentage of worker activities that could be automated (60 to 70 percent, up from 50 percent), this doesn’t necessarily translate into the automation of an entire role.

AI high performers are expected to conduct much higher levels of reskilling than other companies are. Respondents at these organizations are over three times more likely than others to say their organizations will reskill more than 30 percent of their workforces over the next three years as a result of AI adoption.

4. With all eyes on gen AI, AI adoption and impact remain steady

While the use of gen AI tools is spreading rapidly, the survey data doesn’t show that these newer tools are propelling organizations’ overall AI adoption. The share of organizations that have adopted AI overall remains steady, at least for the moment, with 55 percent of respondents reporting that their organizations have adopted AI. Less than a third of respondents continue to say that their organizations have adopted AI in more than one business function, suggesting that AI use remains limited in scope. Product and service development and service operations continue to be the two business functions in which respondents most often report AI adoption, as was true in the previous four surveys. And overall, just 23 percent of respondents say at least 5 percent of their organizations’ EBIT last year was attributable to their use of AI—essentially flat with the previous survey—suggesting there is much more room to capture value.

Organizations continue to see returns in the business areas in which they are using AI, and they plan to increase investment in the years ahead. We see a majority of respondents reporting AI-related revenue increases within each business function using AI. And looking ahead, more than two-thirds expect their organizations to increase their AI investment over the next three years.

The online survey was in the field April 11 to 21, 2023, and garnered responses from 1,684 participants representing the full range of regions, industries, company sizes, functional specialties, and tenures. Of those respondents, 913 said their organizations had adopted AI in at least one function and were asked questions about their organizations’ AI use. To adjust for differences in response rates, the data are weighted by the contribution of each respondent’s nation to global GDP.

The survey content and analysis were developed by Michael Chui , a partner at the McKinsey Global Institute and a partner in McKinsey’s Bay Area office, where Lareina Yee is a senior partner; Bryce Hall , an associate partner in the Washington, DC, office; and senior partners Alex Singla and Alexander Sukharevsky , global leaders of QuantumBlack, AI by McKinsey, based in the Chicago and London offices, respectively.

They wish to thank Shivani Gupta, Abhisek Jena, Begum Ortaoglu, Barr Seitz, and Li Zhang for their contributions to this work.

This article was edited by Heather Hanselman, an editor in the Atlanta office.

Explore a career with us

Related articles.

McKinsey partners Lareina Yee and Michael Chui

The economic potential of generative AI: The next productivity frontier

A green apple split into 3 parts on a gray background. Half of the apple is made out of a digital blue wireframe mesh.

What is generative AI?

Circular hub element virtual reality of big data, technology concept.

Exploring opportunities in the generative AI value chain

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

Working Memory Underpins Cognitive Development, Learning, and Education

Working memory is the retention of a small amount of information in a readily accessible form. It facilitates planning, comprehension, reasoning, and problem-solving. I examine the historical roots and conceptual development of the concept and the theoretical and practical implications of current debates about working memory mechanisms. Then I explore the nature of cognitive developmental improvements in working memory, the role of working memory in learning, and some potential implications of working memory and its development for the education of children and adults. The use of working memory is quite ubiquitous in human thought, but the best way to improve education using what we know about working memory is still controversial. I hope to provide some directions for research and educational practice.

What is Working Memory? An Introduction and Review

Working memory is the small amount of information that can be held in mind and used in the execution of cognitive tasks, in contrast with long-term memory, the vast amount of information saved in one’s life. Working memory is one of the most widely-used terms in psychology. It has often been connected or related to intelligence, information processing, executive function, comprehension, problem-solving, and learning, in people ranging from infancy to old age and in all sorts of animals. This concept is so omnipresent in the field that it requires careful examination both historically and in terms of definition, to establish its key characteristics and boundaries. By weaving together history, a little philosophy, and empirical work in psychology, in this opening section I hope to paint a clear picture of the concept of working memory. In subsequent sections, implications of working memory for cognitive development, learning, and education will be discussed in turn, though for these broad areas it is only feasible to touch on certain examples.

Some researchers emphasize the possibility of training working memory to improve learning and education. In this chapter, I take the complementary view that we must learn how to adjust the materials to facilitate learning and education with the working memory abilities that the learner has. Organizing knowledge, for example, reduces one’s memory load because the parts don’t have to be held in mind independently.

Take, for example, the possibility of doing some scouting ahead so that you will know what this article is about, making your task of reading easier. If you tried to read through the headings of this article, you might have trouble remembering them (placing them all in working memory) so as to anticipate how they fit together. If you read Figure 1 , though, it is an attempt to help you organize the information. If it helps you associate the ideas to one another to build a coherent framework, it should help you read by reducing the working-memory load you experience while reading. In doing so, you are building a rich structure to associate the headings with one another in long-term memory (e.g., Ericsson & Kintsch, 1995 ), which reduces the number of ideas that would have to be held independently in working memory in order to remember the organization.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms545499f1.jpg

Schematic diagram of the arguments in the present article.

Early History of Working Memory Research

In 1690, John Locke distinguished between contemplation, or holding an idea in mind, and memory, or the power to revive an idea after it has disappeared from the mind ( Logie, 1996 ). The holding in mind is limited to a few concepts at once and reflects what is now called working memory, as opposed to the possibly unlimited store of knowledge from a lifetime that is now called long-term memory. Working memory can be defined as the small amount of information that can be held in an especially accessible state and used in cognitive tasks.

Philosophers have long been interested in the limits of what can be contemplated, as noted by a leading British economist and logician, William Stanley Jevons. In an article in Science in 1871, he mused (p. 281): “It is well known that the mind is unable through the eye to estimate any large number of objects without counting them successively. A small number, for instance three or four, it can certainly comprehend and count by an instantaneous and apparently single act of mental attention.” Then he devised a little experiment to test this hypothesis, on himself. On each trial, he casually reached into a jar full of beans, threw several beans onto a table, and tried to estimate their number without counting. After 1,027 trials, he made no errors for sets of 3 or 4 beans, with some small errors for sets of 5 beans, and with increasing magnitudes of error as a function of set size thereafter, up to 15 beans. Despite the problematic nature of the method (in that the bean thrower was also the bean judge), the finding that normal adults typically can keep in mind only about 3 or 4 items has been replicated many times in modern research, using methods similar to Jevons (e.g., Mandler & Shebo, 1982 ) and using many other methods ( Cowan, 2001 ). The limited amount that could be held in mind at once played an important role in early experimental psychology, e.g., in the early experimental work of Hermann Ebbinghaus (1885/1913) and Wilhelm Wundt (1894/1998) . On the American front, William James (1890) wrote about a distinction between primary memory, the items in consciousness and the trailing edge of what is perceived in the world, and secondary memory, the items in storage but not currently in consciousness. Recent investigators have considered multiple possible reasons why primary memory might be limited to just a few items at once, including biological accounts based on the need to avoid confusion between concurrent objects in memory, and evolutionary and teleological accounts based on ideas about what capacity might be ideal for learning and memory retrieval ( Cowan, 2010 ; Sweller, 2011 ), but as yet the reason is unknown.

Ubiquity of the Working Memory Concept

When we say that working memory holds a small amount of information , by this term we may be referring to something as abstract as ideas that can be contemplated, or something as concrete as objects that can be counted (e.g., beans). The main point of information is that it is the choice of some things out of a greater set of possible things. One of the exciting aspects of working memory is that it may be important on so many different levels, and in so many different situations. When you are listening to language, you need to retain information about the beginning of the sentence until you can make sense of it. If you hear Jean would like to visit the third building on the left you need to recall that the actor in the sentence is Jean. Then you need to retain the verb until you know what it is she would like to visit, and you need to retain the adjective “third” until you know, third what; and all of the pieces must be put together in the right way. Without sufficient working memory, the information would be lost before you could combine it into a coherent, complete thought. As another example of how working memory is used, when doing simple arithmetic in your head, if you want to add 24 and 18 you may need to find that 4+8=12, retain the 2 while carrying the 1 over to the tens column to make 2+1+1=4 in the tens column, and integrate with the ones columns to arrive at the answer, 42. As a third example, if you are searching for your car in a parking lot, you have to remember the layout of the cars in the region you just searched so that you can avoid wasting time searching the same region again. In the jungle, a predator that turns its vision away from a scene and revisits it moments later may use working memory to detect that something in the scene has shifted; this change detection may indicate the presence of prey.

So the information in working memory can range from spoken words and printed digits to cars and future meals. It can even encompass abstract ideas. Consider whether a young child can get a good understanding of what is or is not a tiger (a matter of word category concepts, e.g., Nelson, 1974 ; Saltz, Soller, & Sigel, 1972 ). The concept is, in lay terms, a big cat with stripes. It excludes lions, which have no stripes, and it excludes zebras, which are not big cats. The child must be able to keep in mind the notion of a cat and the notion of stripes at the same time in order to grasp the tiger concept correctly. If the child thinks only of the stripes, he or she may incorrectly label a zebra as a tiger. The concept presumably starts out in working memory and, once it is learned, is transferred to long-term memory. At first, an incomplete concept might be stored in long-term memory, leading to misconceptions that are corrected later when discrepancies with further input are noticed and working memory is used to amend the concept in long-term memory. On a more abstract plane, there are more semantic issues mastered somewhat later in childhood (e.g., Clark & Garnica, 1974 ). The concept of bringing something seems to require several conditions: the person doing the bringing must have something at a location other than the speaker’s location (or future planned location), and must accompany that thing to the speaker’s location. You can ask the person to bring a salad to your house, but probably not to take a salad to your house (unless you are not there), and not to send a salad to your house (unless they are not coming along). These conditions can tax working memory. Again, the child’s initial concept transferred from working memory to long-term memory may be incomplete, and amended later when discrepancies with further input are noticed.

Working Memory: The Past 64 Years

There are several modern beginnings for the working memory concept. Hebb (1949) had an outlook on temporary memory that was more neurologically based than the earlier concept of primary memory of James (1890) . He spoke of ideas as mediated by assemblies of cells firing in a specific pattern for each idea or concept, and only a few cell assemblies would be active, with current neural firing, at any moment. This vision has played an important role in the field. An issue that is raised by this work is whether working memory should be identified with all of the active information that can be used in immediate memory tests, whether conscious or not, or whether it should be reserved to describe only the conscious information, more in the flavor of James. Given that working memory is a term usually used to explain behavioral outcomes rather than subjective reports, it is typically not restricted to conscious primary memory (e.g., see Baddeley, 1986 ; Baddeley & Hitch, 1974 ; Cowan, 1988 ). Cowan explicitly suggested that there are two aspects of working memory storage: (1) the activated portion of long-term memory, perhaps corresponding to Hebb’s active cell assemblies, and (2) within that activated portion, a smaller subset of items in the focus of attention. The activated memory would consist of a fragmented soup of all kinds of activated features (sensory, phonological, orthographic, spatial, and semantic), whereas the focus of attention would contain just a few well-integrated items or chunks.

Contributions of George Miller

Miller (1956) discussed the limitation in how many items can be held in immediate memory. In the relevant test procedure, a list of items is seen or heard and immediately afterward (that is, with no imposed retention interval), the list must be repeated verbatim. The ability to do so was said to be limited to about seven chunks, where a chunk is a meaningful unit. For example, the random digit list 582931 may have to be encoded initially as six chunks, one per digit, whereas the sequence 123654 probably can be encoded by most adults as only two chunks (an ascending triplet followed by a descending triplet). Subsequent work has suggested that the number seven is a practical result that emerges on the basis of strategies that participants use and that, when it is not possible to use chunking or covert verbal rehearsal to help performance, adults typically can retain only 3 or 4 pre-existing chunks ( Chen & Cowan, 2009 ; Cowan, 2001 ; Cowan, Rouder, Blume, & Saults, 2012 ; Luck & Vogel, 1997 ; Rouder et al., 2008 ).

The first mention I have found of the term working memory comes from a book by Miller, Galanter, and Pribram (1960) , Plans and the structure of behavior . The title itself, and the concept of organization, seems reminiscent of the earlier work by Hebb (1949) , The organization of behavior . Miller et al. observed that daily functioning in the world requires a hierarchy of plans. For example, your plan to do well at work requires a sub-plan to be there at time in the morning, which in turn may require sub-plans to eat breakfast, shower, get dressed, gather work materials, and so on. Each of these plans also may have sub-plans, and you may have competing plans (such as choosing an after-work activity, calling your mother, or acquiring food for dinner). Our working memory was said to be the mental faculty whereby we remember the plans and sub-plans. We cannot think about all of them at once but we might, for example, keep in mind that the frying pan is hot while retrieving a knife from the drawer, and we may keep bringing to mind the approximate time so as not to be late. Working memory was said to be the facility that is used to carry out one sub-plan while keeping in mind the necessary related sub-plans and the master plan.

Contributions of Donald Broadbent

In Great Britain, Broadbent’s (1958) book helped to bring the conversation out of the behaviorist era and into an era of cognitive psychology. In a footnote within the book, he sketched a rough information processing diagram that showed information progressing from a sensory type of store that holds a lot of information briefly, through an attention filter to essentially a working memory that holds only a few items, to a long-term memory that is our storehouse of knowledge accumulated through a lifetime. The empirical basis for the model came largely from his work with selective attention, including many dichotic listening studies in which the task was to listen to the message from one ear and ignore the message from the other ear, or report both messages in some order. The motivation for this kind of research came largely from practical issues provoked by World War II, such as how to help a pilot listen to his own air traffic control message while ignoring messages meant for other pilots but presented in the same channel. An important theoretical outcome, however, was the discovery of a difference between a large-capacity but short-lived sensory memory that was formed regardless of attention, and a longer-lived but small-capacity abstract working memory that required attention.

Contributions of Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch

Miller et al. (1960) may have devised the term working memory but they were not the predominant instigator of the work that has occurred subsequently in the field. Google Scholar does show it with over 5,600 citations. A chapter by Baddeley and Hitch (1974) , though, is listed with over 7,400 citations and a 1992 Science article summarizing that approach has over 14,500 citations. In the 1974 chapter, the term working memory was used to indicate a system of temporary memory that is multifaceted, unlike the single store such as James’ primary memory, or the corresponding box in Broadbent’s (1958) model, or an elaborated version of it as in the model of Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) model, none of which would do. In fact, a lot of investigators in the 1960’s proposed variations of information processing models that included a single short-term memory store, and Baddeley often has referred to these together, humorously, as the “modal model,” providing a sketch of it with sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory boxes as in the Broadbent and the Atkinson/Shiffrin models. (When the humor and the origin of the phrase “modal model” are forgotten, yet the phrase is still widely used, it seems sad somehow.)

The main point emphasized by Baddeley and Hitch (1974) is that there were diverse effects that appeared to implicate short-term memory, but that did not converge to a single component. Phonological processing interfered most with phonological storage, visual-spatial processing interfered with visual-spatial storage, and a working memory load did not seem to interfere much with superior memory for the end of a list, or recency effect. Conceptual learning did not depend heavily on the type of memory that was susceptible to phonological similarity effects, and a patient with a very low memory span was still able to learn new facts. To account for all of the dissociations, they ended up concluding that there was an attention-related control system and various storage systems. These included a phonological system that also included a covert verbal rehearsal process, and a visual-spatial storage system that might have its own type of non-verbal rehearsal. In the 1974 version of the theory, there were attention limits on the storage of information as well as on processing. In a 1986 book, Baddeley eliminated the attention-dependent storage but in a 2000 paper, a new component was added in the form of an episodic buffer. This buffer might or might not be attention-dependent and is responsible for holding semantic information for the short term, as well as the specific binding or association between phonological and visual-spatial information. Baddeley and Hitch called the assembly or system of storage and processing in service of holding information in an accessible form working memory, the memory one uses in carrying out cognitive tasks of various kinds (i.e., cognitive work).

Model of Cowan (1988)

Through the years, there were several other proposals that alter the flavor of the working memory proposal. Cowan (1988) was concerned with how we represent what we know and do not know about information processing. The “modal models” of which Baddeley has spoken began with Broadbent’s (1958) model in which the boxes were shown to be accessed in sequence, comparable to a computer flow chart: first sensory memory, then an attention filter, then short-term memory, and then long-term memory. Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968) preserved the flow chart structure but added recursive entry into the boxes, in the form of the control processes. Baddeley and Hitch (1974) and Baddeley (1986) instead used a processing diagram in which the boxes could be accessed in parallel. One presumably could enter some information into phonological storage while concurrently entering other information into visual-spatial storage, with interacting modules and concurrent executive control.

Cowan (1988 , 1995 , 1999 , 2001 , 2005 ) recoiled a bit from the modules and separate boxes, partly because they might well form an arbitrarily incomplete taxonomy of the systems in the brain. (Where would spatial information about sound go? Where would touch information go? These types of unanswered questions also may have helped motivate the episodic buffer of Baddeley, 2000 .) There could be multiple modules, but because we do not know the taxonomy, they were all thrown into the soup of activated long-term memory. Instead of separate boxes, I attempted to model on a higher level at which distinctions that were incomplete were not explicitly drawn into the model, and mechanisms could be embedded in other mechanisms. Thus, there was said to be a long-term memory, a subset of which was in an activated state (cf. Hebb, 1949 ), and within that, a smaller subset of which was in the focus of attention (cf. James, 1890 ). Dissociations could still occur on the basis of similarity of features; two items with phonological features will interfere with one another, for example, more than one item with phonological features and another item with only visual-spatial features. The model still included central executive processes.

Compared to Baddeley and Hitch (1974) , Cowan (1988) also placed more emphasis on sensory memory. It is true that printed letters, like spoken letters, are encoded with speech-based, phonological features that can be confused with each other in working memory (e.g., Conrad, 1964 ). Nevertheless, there is abundant other evidence that lists presented in a spoken form are remembered much better, in particular at the end of the list, than verbal lists presented in printed form (e.g., Murdock & Walker, 1969 ; Penney, 1989 ).

The attention filter also was internalized in the model of Cowan (1988) . Instead of information having to pass through a filter, it was assumed that all information activates long-term memory to some degree. The mind forms a neural model of what it has processed. This will include sensory information for all stimuli but, in the focus of attention, much more semantic information than one finds for unattended information. Incoming information that matches the current neural model becomes habituated, but changes that are perceived cause dishabituation in the form of attentional orienting responses toward the dishabituated stimuli (cf. Sokolov, 1963 ). Such a system has properties similar to the attenuated filtering model of Treisman (1960) or the pertinence model of Normal (1968) . Attention is controlled in this view dually, often with a struggle between voluntary executive control and involuntary orienting responses.

How consistent is Cowan (1988) with the Baddeley and Hitch model? Contributions of Robert Logie

With the addition of the episodic buffer, the model of Baddeley and Hitch makes predictions that are often similar to those of Cowan (1988) . There still may be important differences, though. An open question is whether the activated portion of long-term memory of Cowan (1988) functionally serves the same purpose as the phonological and visual-spatial buffers of Baddeley and Hitch (1974) and Baddeley (1986) . Robert Logie and colleagues argue that this cannot be, inasmuch as visual imagery and visual short-term memory are dissociated ( Borst, Niven, & Logie, 2012 ; Logie & van der Meulen, 2009 ; van der Meulen, Logie, & Della Sala, 2009 ). Irrelevant visual materials interfere with the formation of visual imagery but not with visual storage, whereas tapping in a spatial pattern interferes with visual storage but not the formation of visual images. According to the model that these sources put forward, visual imagery involves activation of long-term memory representations, whereas visual short-term storage is a separate buffer. Although this is a possibility that warrants further research, I am not yet convinced. There could be other reasons for the dissociation. For example, in the study of van der Meulen et al., the visual imagery task involved detecting qualities of the letters presented (curved line or not, enclosed space or not, etc.) and these qualities could overlap more with the picture interference; whereas the visual memory task involved remembering letters in upper and lower case visually, in the correct serial order, and the serial order property may suffer more interference from tapping in a sequential spatial pattern. Testing of the generality of the effects across tasks with different features is needed.

Other models of cross-domain generality

One difference between the Baddeley (1986) framework and that of Cowan (1988) was that Cowan placed more emphasis on the possibility of interference between domains. There has been a continuing controversy about the extent to which verbal and nonverbal codes in working memory interfere with one another (e.g., Cocchini, Logie, Della Sala, MacPherson, & Baddeley, 2002 ; Cowan & Morey, 2007 ; Fougnie & Marois, 2011 ; Morey & Bieler, 2013 ). The domain-general view has extended to other types of research. Daneman and Carpenter (1980) showed that reading and remembering words are tasks that interfere with one another, with the success of remembering in the presence of reading a strong correlate of reading comprehension ability. Engle and colleagues (e.g., Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999 ; Kane et al., 2004 ) showed that this sort of effect does not just occur with verbal materials, but occurs even with storage and processing in separate domains, such as spatial recall with verbal memory. They attributed individual differences primarily to the processing tasks and the need to hold in mind task instructions and goals while suppressing irrelevant distractions.

Barrouillet and colleagues (e.g., Barrouillet, Portrat, & Camos, 2011 ; Vergauwe, Barrouillet, & Camos, 2010 ) emphasized that the process of using attention to refresh items, no matter whether verbal or nonverbal in nature, takes time and counteracts decay. They provided complex tasks involving concurrent storage and processing, like Daneman and Carpenter and like Engle and colleagues. The key measure is cognitive load, the proportion of time that is taken up by the processing task rather than being free for the participant to use to refresh the representations of items to be remembered. The finding of Barrouillet and colleagues has been that the effect of cognitive load on the length of list that can be recalled, or memory span, is a negative linear (i.e., deleterious) effect. They do also admit that there is a verbal rehearsal process that is separate from attentional refreshing, with the option of using either mode of memory maintenance depending on the task demands ( Camos, Mora, & Oberauer, 2011 ), but there is more emphasis on attentional refreshing than in the case of Baddeley and colleagues, and the approach therefore seems more in keeping with Cowan (1988) with its focus of attention (regarding refreshing see also Cowan, 1992 ).

Ongoing controversies about the nature of working-memory memory limits

There are theoretically two basic ways in which working memory could be more limited than long-term memory. First, It could be limited in terms of how many items can be held at once, a capacity limit that Cowan (1998, 2001 ) tentatively ascribes to the focus of attention. Second, it could be limited in the amount of time for which an item remains in working memory when it is no longer rehearsed or refreshed, a decay limit that Cowan (1988) ascribed to the activated portion of long-term memory, the practical limit being up to about 30 seconds depending on the task.

Both of these limits are currently controversial. Regarding the capacity limit, there is not much argument that, within a particular type of stimulus coding (phonological, visual-spatial, etc.), normal adults are limited to about 3 or 4 meaningful units or chunks. The debate is whether the limit occurs in the focus of attention, or because materials of similar sorts interfere with one another (e.g., Oberauer, Lewandowsky, Farrell, Jarrold, & Greaves, 2012 ). In my recent, still-unpublished work, I suggest that the focus of attention is limited to several chunks of information, but that these chunks can be off-loaded to long-term memory and held there, with the help of some attentional refreshing, while the focus of attention is primarily used to encode additional information.

Regarding the memory loss or decay limit, some studies have shown no loss of information for lists of printed verbal materials across periods in which rehearsal and refreshing have apparently been prevented ( Lewandowsky, Duncan, & Brown, 2004 ; Oberauer & Lewandowsky, 2008 ). Nevertheless, for arrays of unfamiliar characters followed by a mask to eliminate sensory memory, Ricker and Cowan (2010) did find memory loss or decay (cf. Zhang & Luck, 2009 ). In further work, Ricker et al. (in press) suggested that the amount of decay depends on how well the information is consolidated in working memory (cf. Jolicoeur & Dell'Acqua, 1998 ). Given that the time available for refreshing appeared to be inversely related to the cognitive load, the consolidation process that seems critical is not interrupted by a mask but continues after it. This consolidation process could be some sort of strengthening of the episodic memory trace based on attentional refreshing in the spirit of Barrouillet et al. (2011) . If so, the most important effect of this refreshing would not be to reverse the effects of decay temporarily, as Barrouillet et al. proposed, but rather to alter the rate of decay itself. Our plans for future research include investigation of these possibilities.

Long-term working memory

It is clear that people function quite well in complex environments in which detailed knowledge must be used in an expert manner, despite a severe limit in working memory to a few ideas or items at once. What is critical in understanding this paradox of human performance is that each slot in working memory can be filled with a concept of great complexity, provided that the individual has the necessary knowledge in long-term memory. This point was made by Miller (1956) in his concept of combining items to form larger chunks of information, with the limit in working memory found in the number of chunks, not the number of separate items presented for memorization. Ericsson and Kintsch (1995) took this concept further by expanding the definition of working memory to include relevant information in long-term memory.

Although we might quibble about the best definition of working memory, it seems undeniable that long-term memory is often used as Ericsson and Kintsch (1995) suggest. An example is what happens when one is holding a conversation with a visitor that is interrupted by a telephone call. During the call, the personal conversation with one’s visitor is typically out of conscious working memory. After the call, however, with the visitor serving as a vivid cue, it is often possible to retrieve a memory of the conversation as a recent episode and to remember where this conversation left off. That might not be possible some days later. This use of long-term memory to serve a function similar to the traditional working memory, thus expanding the person’s capabilities, was termed long-term working memory by Ericsson and Kintsch. Cowan (1995) alluded to a similar use of long-term memory for this purpose but, not wanting to expand the definition of working memory, called the function virtual short-term memory, meaning a use of long-term memory in a way that short-term memory is usually used. It is much like the use of computer memory that allows the computer to be turned off in hibernation mode and later returned to its former state when the memory is retrieved.

Given the ability of humans to use long-term memory so adeptly, one could ask why we care about the severe working memory capacity limit at all. The answer is that it is critical when there is limited long-term knowledge of the topic. In such circumstances, the capacity of working memory can determine how many items can be held in mind at once in order to use the items together, or to link them to form a new concept in long-term memory. This is the case in many situations that are important for learning and comprehension. One simple example of using items together is following a set of instructions, e.g., to a preschool child, put your drawing in your cubby and then go sit in the circle . Part of that instruction may be forgotten before it is carried out and teachers must be sensitive to that possibility. A simple example of linking items together is in reading a novel, when one listens to a description of a character and melds the parts of the description to arrive at an overall personality sketch that can be formed in long-term memory. Inadequate use of working memory during reading may lead to the sketch being incomplete, as some descriptive traits are inadvertently ignored. Knowledge of this working memory limit can be used to improve one’s writing by making it easier to remember and comprehend.

Paas and Sweller (2012) bring up the distinction between biologically primary and secondary knowledge ( Geary, 2008 ) and suggest (p. 29) that “Humans are easily able to acquire huge amounts of biologically primary knowledge outside of educational contexts and without a discernible working memory load.” Examples they offered were the learning of faces and learning to speak. It may well be the case that individual faces or spoken words quickly become integrated chunks in long-term memory (and, I would add, the same seems true for objects in domains of learned expertise, e.g., written words in adults). Nevertheless, the biologically-primary components are used in many situations in which severe capacity limits do apply. In these situations, the added memory demand is considered biologically secondary. An example is learning which face should be associated with which name. If four novel faces are shown on a screen and their names are vocally presented, these name-face pairs cannot be held in working memory at once, so it is difficult to retain the information and it often takes additional study of one pair at a time to remember the name-face pairing.

Specific mathematical models

Here I have been selective in examining models of working memory that are rather overarching and verbally specified. By limiting the domain of applicability and adding some processing assumptions, other researchers throughout the years have been able to formulate models that make mathematical predictions of performance in specific situations. We have learned a lot from them but they are essentially outside of the scope of this review given limited space and given my own limitations. For examples of such models see Brown, Neath, & Chater, 2007 ; Burgess & Hitch, 1999 ; Cowan et al., 2012 ; Farrell & Lewandowsky, 2002 ; Hensen, 1998 ; Murdock, 1982; Oberauer & Lewandowsky, 2011 ). The importance of these models is that they make clear the consequences of our theoretical assumptions. In order to make quantitative predictions, each mathematical assumption must be made explicit. It is sometimes found that the effects of certain proposed mechanisms, taken together, are not what one might assume from a purely verbal theory. Of course, some of the assumptions that one must make to eke out quantitative predictions may be unsupported, so I believe that the best way forward in the field is to use general verbal, propositional thinking some of the time and specific quantitative modeling other times, working toward a convergence of these methods toward a common theory.

Summary: Status of Working Memory

The progress in this field might be likened to an upward spiral. We make steady progress but meanwhile, we go in circles. The issues of the nature of working memory limits have not changed much from the early days. Why is the number of items limited? Why is the duration limited? What makes us forget? How is it related to the conscious mind and to neural processes? These questions are still not answered. At the same time, we have agreement about what can be found in particular circumstances. Set up the stimuli one way, and there is interference between modalities. Set it up another way and there appears to be much less interference. Set it up one way and items are lost rapidly across time. Set it up a different way, and there is much less loss. There are brain areas associated with the focus of attention and with working memory across modalities ( Cowan, 2011 ; Cowan, Li et al., 2011 ; Todd & Marois, 2004 ; Xu & Chun, 2006 ). This is progress awaiting an adequate unifying theory.

What we do know has practical implications. To avoid overtaxing an individual’s working-memory capabilities, one should avoid presenting more than a few items or ideas at once, unless the items can be rapidly integrated. One should also avoid making people hold on to unintegrated information for a very long time. For example, I could write a taxing sentence like, It is said that, if your work is not overwhelming, your car is in good repair, and the leaves have changed color, it is a good time for a fall vacation . However, that sentence requires a lot from the reader’s working memory. I could reduce the working memory load by not making you wait for the information that provides the unifying theme, keeping the working memory load low: It is said that a good time for a fall vacation is when your work is not overwhelming, your car is in good repair, and the leaves have changed color .

Working Memory and Cognitive Development

There is no question that working-memory capabilities increase across the life span of the individual. In early tests of maturation (e.g., Bolton, 1892 ), and to this day in tests of intelligence, children have been asked to repeat lists of random digits. The length of list that can be successfully repeated on some predefined proportion of trials is the digit span. It increases steadily with childhood maturation, until late childhood. When the complexity of the task is increased, the time to adult-like performance is extended a bit further, with steady improvement throughout childhood (for an example see Gathercole, Pickering, Ambridge, & Wearing, 2004 ).

As we saw in the introductory section, clear practical findings do not typically come with a clear understanding of the theoretical explanation. There have been many explanations over the years for the finding of increasing memory span with age (e.g., see Bauer & Fivush, in press ; Courage & Cowan, 2009 ; Kail, 1990 ). These explanations may lead to differing opinions of the best course for learning and education, as well.

Explanations Based on Capacity

Explanations of intellectual growth based on working memory capacity stem from what has been called the neoPiagetian school of thought. Jean Piaget outlined a series of developmental stages, but with no known underlying reason for the progression between stages. Pascual-Leone and Smith (1969) attributed the developmental increases to increases in the number of items that could be held in mind at once.

The theory becomes more explicit with the contributions of Halford, Phillips, and Wilson (1998) and Andrews and Halford (2002) . They suggests that it is the number of associations between elements that is restricted and that this matters because it limits the complexity of thought. In my example above, the concept of a tiger versus lion versus zebra requires concurrent consideration of the animal’s shape and presence or absence of stripes. Similarly, addition requires the association between three elements: the two elements being added and the sum. A concept like bigger than is a logical relation requiring three slots, e.g., bigger than (dog, elephant) . Ratios require the coordination of four elements (e.g., 4/6 is equivalent to 6/9) and therefore are considerably harder to grasp, according to the theory (see Halford, Cowan, & Andrews, 2007 ).

This concept is quite promising and might even appear to be “the only game in town” when it comes to trying to understand the age limits on children’s ability to comprehend ideas of various levels of complexity. One problem with it is that it is not always straightforward to determine the arity of a concept, or number of ideas that must be associated. For example, a young child might understand the concept big(elephant) and then might be able to infer that elephants are bigger than dogs, without being able to use the concept of bigger than in a consistent manner more generally. The concept from Miller (1956) that items can be combined using knowledge to form larger chunks also applies to associations, and it is not clear how to be sure that the level of complexity actually is what it is supposed to be. Knowledge allows some problems to be solved with less working memory requirement.

Explanations Based on Knowledge

It is beyond question that knowledge increases with age. Perhaps this knowledge increase is the sole reason for developmental change in working memory, it has been argued. Chi (1978) showed that children with an expertise in the game of chess could remember chess configurations better than adults with no such expertise. The expert children presumably could form larger chunks of chess pieces, greatly reducing the memory load. Case, Kurland, and Goldberg (1982) gave adults materials that were unfamiliar and found that both the speed of item identification and the memory span for those materials closely resembled what was found for 6-year-olds on familiar materials. The implication was that the familiarity with the materials determines the processing speed, which in turn determines the span.

Explanations Based on Processing Speed and Strategies

Case et al. (1982) talked of a familiarity difference leading to a speed difference. Others have suggested that, more generally, speed of processing increases with age in childhood and decrease again with old age (e.g., Kail & Salthouse, 1994 ). This has led to accounts of working memory improvement based on an increased rate of covert verbal rehearsal ( Hulme & Tordoff, 1989 ) or increased rate of attentional refreshing ( Barrouillet, Gavens, Vergauwe, Gaillard, & Camos, 2009 ; Camos & Barrouillet, 2011 ). At the lower end of childhood, it has been suggested on the basis of various evidence that young children do not rehearse at all ( Flavell, Beach, & Chinsky, 1966 ; Garrity, 1975 ; Henry, 1991 ) or do not rehearse in a sufficiently sophisticated manner that is needed to assist in recall ( Ornstein & Naus, 1978 ). When rehearsal aloud is required, the result suggest that the most recently rehearsed items are recalled best ( Tan & Ward, 2000 ).

This view that rehearsal is actually important has been opposed recently. It is not clear that rehearsal must be invoked to explain performance ( Jarrold & Citroën, 2013 ) and if rehearsal takes place, it is not clear exactly what the internal processes are (e.g., cumulative repetition of the list? Repetition of each item as it is presented?).

In the case of using attention to refresh information, an interesting case can be made. Children who are too young (about 4 years of age and younger) do not seem to use attention to refresh items. For them, the limit in performance depends on the duration of the retention interval. For older children and adults, who are able to refresh, it is not the absolute duration but the cognitive load that determines performance ( Barrouillet et al., 2011 ). The “phase change” in performance that is observed here with the advent of refreshing is perhaps comparable to the phase change that is seen with the advent of verbal rehearsal ( Henry, 1991 ), though the evidence may be stronger in the case of refreshing.

Re-assessment of Capacity Accounts

We have seen that there are multiple ways in which children’s working memory performance gets better with maturity. There are reasons to care about whether the growth of capacity is primary, or whether it is derived from some other type of development. For example, if the growth of capacity results only from the growth of knowledge, then it should be possible to teach any concept at any age, if the concept can be made familiar enough. If capacity differences come from speed differences, it might be possible to allow more time by making sure that the parts to be incorporated into a new concept are presented sufficiently slowly.

We have done a number of experiments suggesting that there is something to capacity that changes independent of these other factors. Regarding knowledge, relevant evidence was provided by Cowan, Nugent, Elliott, Ponomarev, and Saults (1999) in their test of memory for digits that were unattended while a silent picture-rhyming game was carried out. The digits were attended only occasionally, when a recall cue was presented about 1 s after the last digit. The performance increase with age throughout the elementary school years was just as big for small digits (1, 2, 3), which are likely to be familiar, as for large digits (7, 8, 9), which are less familiar. Gilchrist, Cowan, and Naveh-Benjamin (2009) further examined memory for lists of unrelated, spoken sentences in order to distinguish between a measure of capacity and a measure of linguistic knowledge. The measure of capacity was an access rate, the number of sentences that were at least partly recalled. The measure of linguistic knowledge was a completion rate, the proportion of a sentence that was recalled, provided that at least part of it was recalled. This sentence completion rate was about 80% for both first and sixth grader children, suggesting that for these simple sentences, there was no age difference in knowledge. Nevertheless, the number of sentences accessed was considerably smaller in first-grade children than in sixth-grade children (about 2.5 sentences vs. 4 sentences). I conclude, tentatively at least, that knowledge differences cannot account for the age difference in working memory capacity.

We have used a different procedure to help rule out a number of factors that potentially could underlie the age difference in observed capacity. It is based on a procedure that has been well-researched in adults ( Luck & Vogel, 1997 ). On each trial of this procedure, an array of simple items (such as colored squares) is presented briefly and followed by a retention interval of about 1 s, and then a single probe item is presented. The latter is to be judged identical to the array item from the same location, or a new item. This task is convenient partly because there are mathematical ways to estimate the number of items in working memory ( Cowan, 2001 ). If k items are in working memory and there are N items in the array, the likelihood that the probed item is known is k/N , and a correct response can also come from guessing. It is possible to calculate k , which for this procedure is equal to N ( h-f ), where h refers the proportion of change trials in which the change was detected (hits) and f refers to the proportion of no-change trials in which a change was incorrectly reported (false alarms).

One possibility is that younger children remember less of the requested information because they attend to more irrelevant information, cluttering working memory (for adults, cf. Vogel, McCollough, & Machizawa, 2005 ). To examine this, Cowan, Morey, AuBuchon, Zwilling, and Gilchrist (2010) presented both colored circles and colored triangles and instructed participants to pay closer attention to one shape, which was tested on 80% of the trials in critical blocks. When there were 2 triangles and 2 circles, memory for the more heavily-attended shape was better than memory for the less-attended shape, to the same extent in children in Grades 1–2 and Grades 6–7, and in college students. Yet, the number of items in working memory was much lower in children in Grades 1–2 than in the two older groups. It did not seem that the inability to filter out irrelevant information accounted for the age difference in capacity.

Another possibility is that in Cowan et al. (2010) , the array items occurred too fast for the younger children to encode correctly. To examine this, Cowan, AuBuchon, Gilchrist, Ricker, & Saults (2011) presented the items one at a time at relatively slow, a 1-item-per-second rate. The results remained the same as before. In some conditions, the participant had to repeat each color as it was presented or else say “wait” to suppress rehearsal; this articulatory manipulation, too, left the developmental effect unchanged. It appears that neither encoding speed nor articulation could account for the age differences. So we believe that age differences in capacity may be primary rather than derived from another process.

Age differences in capacity still could occur because of age differences in the speed of a rapid process of refreshment, and from the absence of refreshment in young children ( Camos & Barrouillet, 2011 ). Alternatively, it could occur because of age differences in some other type of speed, neural space, or efficiency. This remains to be seen but at least we believe that there is a true maturational change in working memory capacity underlying age differences in the ability to comprehend materials of different complexity. This is in addition to profound effects of knowledge acquisition and the ability to use strategies.

The use of strategies themselves may be secondary to the available working memory resources to carry out those strategies. According to the neoPiagetian view of Pascual-Leone and Smith (1969) , for example, the tasks themselves share resources with the data being stored. Cowan et al. (2010) found that when the size of the array to be remembered was large (3 more-relevant and 3 less-relevant items, rather than 2 of each) then young children were no longer able to allocate more attention to the more-relevant items. The attentional resource allocated to the items in the array was apparently deducted from the resource available to allocate attention optimally.

In practical terms, it is worth remembering that several aspects of working memory are likely to develop: capacity, speed, knowledge, and the use of strategies. Although it is not always easy to know which process is primary, these aspects of development all should contribute in some way to our policies regarding learning and education.

Working Memory and Learning

In early theories of information processing, up through the current period, working memory was viewed as a portal to long-term memory. In order for information to enter long-term memory in a form that allows later retrieval, it first must be present in working memory in a suitable form. Sometimes that form appears modality-specific. For example, Baddeley, Papagno, and Vallar (1988) wondered how it could be that a patient with a very small verbal short-term memory span, 2 or 3 digits at most, could function so well in most ways and exhibit normal learning capabilities. The answer turned out to be that she displayed a very selective deficit: she was absolutely unable to learn new vocabulary. This finding led to a series of developmental studies showing that individual differences in phonological memory are quite important for differences in word-learning capability in both children and adults ( Baddeley, Gathercole, & Papagno, 1998 ; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989 , 1990 ).

Aside from this specific domain, there are several ways in which working memory can influence learning. It is important to have sufficient working memory for concept formation. The control processes and mnemonic strategies used with working memory also are critical to learning.

Working Memory and Concept Formation

Learning might be thought of in an educational context as the formation of new concepts. These new concepts occur when existing concepts are joined or bound together. Some of this binding is mundane. If an individual knows what the year 1776 means and also what the Declaration of Independence is (at least in enough detail to remember the title of the declaration), then it is possible to learn the new concept that the Declaration of Independence was written in the year 1776. Other times, the binding of concepts may be more interesting and there may be a new conceptual leap involved. For example, a striped cat is a tiger. As another simple example, to understand what a parallelogram is, the child has to understand what the word parallel means, and further to grasp that two sets of parallel lines intersect with one another. The ideas presumably must co-exist in working memory for the concept to be formed.

For the various types of concept formation, then, the cauldron is assumed to be working memory. According to my own view, the binding of ideas occurs more specifically in the focus of attention. We have taken a first step toward verifying that hypothesis. Cowan, Donnell, and Saults (in press) presented lists of words with an incidental task: to report the most interesting word in each presented list. Later, participants completed a surprise test in which they were asked whether pairs of words came from the same list; the words were always one or two serial positions apart in their respective lists, but sometimes were from the same list and sometimes from different lists. The notion was that the link between the words in the same list would be formed only if the words had been in the focus of attention at the same time, which was much more likely for short lists than for long lists. In keeping with this hypothesis, performance was better for words from short lists of 3 items (about 59%) than for words from lists of 6 or 9 items (about 53%). This is a small effect, but it is still important that there was unintentional learning of the association between items that were together in the focus of attention just once, when there was no intention of learning the association.

The theory of Halford et al. (1998) may be the best articulated theory suggesting why a good working memory is important for learning. (In this discussion, a “good” working memory is simply one that can keep in mind sufficient items and their relations to one another to solve the problem at hand, which may require a sufficient combination of capacity, speed, knowledge, and available strategies.) More complex concepts require that one consider the relationship between more parts. A person’s working memory can be insufficient for a complex concept. It may be possible to memorize that concept with less working memory, but not truly to understand the concept and work with it. Take, for example, use of the concept of transitivity in algebra. If a+b=c+d and c+d=e , then we can conclude that a+b=e because equality is transitive. Yet, a person who understands the rules of algebra still would not be able to draw the correct inference if that person could not concurrently remember the two equations. Even if the equations are side by side on the page, that does not mean that they necessarily can be encoded into working memory at the same time, which is necessary in order to draw the inference. Lining up the equations vertically for the learner and then inviting the learner to apply the rule by rote is a method that can be used to reduce the working memory load, perhaps allowing the problem to be solved. However, working out the problem that way will not necessarily produce the insight needed to set up a new problem and solve it, because setting up the problem correctly requires the use of working memory to understand what should be lined up with what. So if the individual does not have sufficient working memory capacity, a rote method of solution may be helpful for the time being. More importantly, though, the problem could be set up in a more challenging manner so that the learner is in the position of having to use his or her working memory to store the information. By doing so, the hope is that successful solution of the problem then will result in more insight that allows the application of the principles to other problems. That, in fact, is an expression of the issues that may lead to the use of word problems in mathematics education.

Working Memory and Control Processes

Researchers appear to be in fairly good agreement that one of the most important aspects of learning is staying on task. If one does not stick to the relevant goals, one will learn something perhaps, but it will not be the desired learning. Individuals who test well on working memory tasks involving a combination of storage and processing have been shown to do a better job staying on task.

A good experimental example of how staying on task is tied to working memory is one carried out by Kane and Engle (2003) using a well-known task designed long ago by John Ridley Stroop. In the key condition within this task, one is to name the color of ink in which color words are written. Sometimes, the color of ink does not match the written color and there is a tendency to want to read the word instead of naming the color. This effect can be made more treacherous by presenting stimuli in which the word and color match on most trials, so that the participant may well lapse into reading and lose track of the correct task goal (naming the color of ink). What that happens, the result is an error or long delay on the occasional trials for which the word and ink do not match. Under those circumstances, the individuals who are more affected by the Stroop conditions are those with relatively low performance on the operation span test of working memory (carrying out arithmetic problems while remembering words interleaved with those problems).

In more recent work, Kane et al. (2007) has shown that low-span individuals have more problems attending in daily life. Participants carried devices that allowed them to respond at unpredictable times during the day, reporting what they were doing, what they wanted to be doing, and so on. It was found that low-span individuals were more likely to report that their minds were wandering away from the tasks on which they were trying to focus attention. This, however, did not occur on all tasks. The span-related difference in attending was only for tasks in which they reported that they wanted to pay attention. When participants reported that they were bored and did not want to pay attention, mind-wandering was just as prevalent for high spans as for low spans.

Although this work was done on adults, it has implications for children as well. Gathercole, Lamont, and Alloway (2006) suggest that working memory failures appear to be a large part of learning disabilities. Children who were often accused of not trying to follow directions tested out as children with low working memory ability. They were often either not able to remember instructions or not able to muster the resources to stick to the task goal and pay attention continually, for the duration needed. Children with various kinds of learning and language disability generally test below grade level on working memory procedures, and children with low working memory and executive function don’t do well in school (e.g., Sabol & Pianta, 2012 ).

Of course, central executive processes must do more than just maintain the task goal. The way in which information is converted from one form to another, the vigilance with which the individual searches for meaningful connections between elements and new solutions, and self-knowledge about what areas are strong or weak all probably play important roles in learning.

Working Memory and Mnemonic Strategies

There also are special strategies that are needed for learning. For example, a sophisticated rehearsal strategy for free recall of a list involves a rehearsal method that is cumulative. If the first word on the list is a cow, the second is a fish, and the third a stone, one ideally should rehearse cumulatively: cow…cow, fish….cow, fish, stone … and so on ( Ornstein & Naus, 1978 ). Cowan, Saults, Winterowd, and Sherk (1991) showed that young children did not carry out cumulative rehearsal the way older children do and could not easily be trained to do so, but that their memory improved when cumulative rehearsal was overtly supported by cumulative presentation of stimuli.

For long-term learning, maintenance rehearsal is not nearly as effective a strategy as elaborative rehearsal, in which a coherent story is made on the basis of the items; this takes time but results in richer associations between items, enhancing long-term memory provided that there is time for it to be accomplished (e.g., Craik & Watkins, 1973 ).

In addition to verbal and elaborative rehearsal, Barrouillet and colleagues (2011) have discussed attentional refreshing as a working-memory maintenance process. We do not yet know what refreshing looks like on a moment-to-moment basis or what implications this kind of maintenance strategy has for long-term learning. It is a rich area for future research.

The most general mnemonic strategy is probably chunking ( Miller, 1956 ), the formation of new associations or recognition of existing ones in order to reduce the number of independent items to keep track of in working memory. The power of chunking is seen in special cases in which individuals have learned to go way beyond the normal performance. Ericsson, Chase, and Faloon (1980) studied an individual who learned, over the course of a year, to repeat lists of about 80 digits from memory. He learned to do so starting with a myriad of athletic records that he knew so that, for example, 396 might be recoded as a single unit, 3.96 minutes, a fairly fast time for running the mile. After applying this intensive chunking strategy in practice for a year, a list of 80 digits could be reduced to several sub-lists, each with associated sub-parts. The idea would be that the basic capacity has not changed but each working-memory slot is filled with quite a complex chunk. In support of this explanation, individuals of this sort still remain at base level (about 7 items) for lists of items that were not practiced in this way, e.g., letters. (For a conceptual replication see Ericsson, Delaney, Weaver, & Mahadevan, 2004 ; Wilding, 2001 )

Although we cannot all reach such great heights of expert performance, we can do amazing things using expertise. For example, memorization of a song or poem is not like memorization of a random list of digits because there are logical connections between the words and between the lines. A little working memory then can go a long way.

The importance of a good working memory comes in when something new is learned, and logical connections are not yet formed so the working memory load is high. When there are not yet sufficient associations between the elements of a body of material, working memory is taxed until the material can be logically organized into a coherent structure. Working memory is thought to correlate most closely with fluid intelligence, the type of intelligence that involves figuring out solutions to new problems (e.g., Wilhelm & Engle, 2005 ). However, crystallized intelligence, the type of intelligence that involves what you know, also is closely related to fluid intelligence. The path I suggest here is that a good working memory assists in problem-solving (hence fluid intelligence); fluid intelligence and working memory then assist in new learning (hence crystallized intelligence).

Working Memory and Education

We have sketched the potential relation between working memory and learning. How is that to be translated into lessons for education? There is a large and diverse literature on this topic. As a starting point to illustrate this diversity, I will describe the chapters chosen for the book, Working memory and education ( Pickering, 2006 ). After an introductory chapter on working memory (A. Baddeley), the book includes two chapters on the relation between working memory and reading (one by P. de Jong and another by K. Cain). There is a chapter on the relation between working memory and mathematics education (R. Bull and K.A. Espy), learning disabilities (H.L. Swanson), attention disorders (K. Cornish and colleagues), and deafness (M. Keehner & J. Atkinson). Other chapters cover more general topics, including the role of working memory in the classroom (S. Gathercole and colleagues), the way to assess working memory in children (S. Pickering), and sources of working memory deficit (M. Minear and P. Shah). It is clear that many avenues of research relate working memory to education, and I cannot travel along all of them in this review.

To organize a diverse field, what I can do is to distinguish between several different basic approaches have been tried. First, one can try to teach to the level of the learner’s working memory. The points described in the article up to this point should be kept in mind when one is trying to discern and understand what a particular learner can and cannot do. Second, one can try to use training exercises to improve working memory, which, investigators have hoped, would allow a person to be able to learn more and solve problems more successfully. The message I would give here is to be wary, given the rudimentary state of the evidence in a difficult field and the plethora of companies selling working memory training exercises. Third, one might contemplate the role of working memory for the most critical goals of education, in a broad sense. These topics will be examined one at a time.

Teaching to the Level of Working Memory

The classic adaptation of education to cognitive development and the needs of learning has been to try to adjust the materials to fit the learner. For example, there has been considerable discussion of the need to delay teaching concepts of arithmetic at least until the children understand the basic underlying concept of one-to-one correspondence; that is, the idea that there are different numbers in a series and that each number is assigned to just one object, in order to count the objects (e.g., Gelman, 1982 ). Halford et al. (2007) provide rough description of what complexity of concepts to expect for each age range, based on working-memory limits (see also Pascual-Leone & Johnson, 2011 ).

There also are individual differences within an age group in ability that affect how the materials are processed. For example, individuals lower in working memory may prefer to take in information using a verbatim, shallow, or surface processing strategy, rather than try to extract the gist (for one relevant investigation, albeit with mixed results, see Kyndt, Cascallar, & Dochy, 2012 ). The enjoyment of technological presentations may be greater in students with better abilities in the most relevant types of working memory (e.g., Garcia, Nussbaum, & Preiss, 2011 ). I would note that the educational enterprise requires that the teacher must decide whether it is best to allow the learner to use a favored strategy, which may be influenced by the student’s ability level, or whether it is possible in some cases to instill a more effective strategy even if it does not come naturally to the student.

Sweller and colleagues ( Sweller, 2011 ; Sweller, van Merrienboer, & Paas, 1998 ) have summarized a body of research literature and a theory about the role of cognitive load in learning and education. Their cognitive load theory is “a theory that emphasizes working memory constraints as determinants of instructional design effectiveness” ( Sweller et al., 1998 ). The theory distinguishes between an intrinsic cognitive load that comes from material to be learned and an extraneous cognitive load that should be kept small enough that the cognitive resources of the learner are not overly depleted by it. The theory is importantly placed in an evolutionary framework that I will not describe (though above I mentioned the theory’s incorporation of the distinction between biologically primary and secondary information). This theory has the advantage of being rather nuanced in that many ramifications of cognitive load are considered. With too high a cognitive load, one runs the risk of the student not being able to follow the presentation, whereas with too low a cognitive load, one runs the risk of insufficient engagement. In future, it might be possible to refine the predictions for classroom learning by combining cognitive load theory with theories of cognitive development, which make some specific predictions about how much capacity is present at a particular age in childhood (e.g., Halford et al., 2007 ). For further discussion of the theory as applied specifically to multimedia, see Schüler, Scheiter, and Genuchten (2011) . Issues arise as to how printed items are encoded (visually, verbally, or both) and how much the combination of verbal and visual codes in multimedia should be expected to tax a common, central cognitive resource and therefore interfere with one another, even when they are intended to be synergic. Both in cognitive psychology and in education, these are key issues currently under ongoing investigation.

An advantage of multimedia and computerized instruction is the possibility of adjusting the instruction to the student’s level. This might be done partly on the basis of success; if the student succeeds, the materials can be made more challenging whereas, if the student fails, the materials can be made easier. One potential pitfall to watch for is that, while some students will want to press slightly beyond their zone of comfort and will learn well, others will want an easy time, and may choose to learn less than they would be capable of learning. One way to cope with these issues is through computerized instruction, but with a heavy dose of personal monitoring and adjustment to make sure that the task is sufficiently motivating for every student.

One factor that makes it difficult to teach to the students effectively is that the working memory demands of language production do not always match the demands of the recipients’ language comprehension. Consequently, when one is speaking or writing for didactic purposes, one must be careful to consider not only one’s own working memory needs, but also those of the listener or reader. There are several obstacles in this regard. Slevc (2011) showed that speakers tend to blurt out what is most readily available in working memory. He used situations that were to be described verbally by the participant, e.g., A pirate gave a book to the monk . If one piece of information had already been presented, it was more likely to be described first. For example, if the monk had been presented already but not the book, the participant was more likely to phrase the description differently, as A pirate gave the monk a book . This assignment of priority to given information is generally appropriate, given that the speaker and listener (or writer and reader) share the same given information. In this case, though, Slevc shows that the tendency to describe given information first was diminished when the speaking participant was under a working memory load. In a didactic situation such as giving a lecture, it thus seems plausible that the memory load inherent in the situation (remembering and planning what one wants to say in the coming segments of a lecture) may cause the lecturer sometimes to use awkward grammatical structure. Moreover, as mentioned above, learning to speak or write well requires that one bear in mind possible difference between what one knows as the speaker (or writer) and what the listener (or reader) knows at key moments. For example, if one says, “Marconi was the inventor of the modern radio,” then, by the time the full topic of the sentence is known, the name is most likely no longer in the listener’s or student’s working memory. If, however, one says, “The modern radio was invented by a man named Marconi,” the context is set up first, making it easier to retain the name. Bearing in mind what the listener or reader knows and does not yet know is likely to be important both for educators in their own speaking and writing, and also in order to teach students how to speak and write effectively.

Working Memory Training

A much more controversial approach is to use training regimens to improve working memory, thereby improving performance on the educational learning tasks that require working memory (e.g., Klingberg, 2010 ). It is controversial partly because many people have spent a great deal of money purchasing such training programs before the scientific community has reached an agreement about the efficacy of such programs.

Doing working memory training studies is not easy. One needs a control group that is just as motivated by the task as the training group but without the working memory training aspect. The training task must be adaptive (with rewards for performance that continues to improve with training) and a non-adaptive control group does not adequately control arousal and motivation. Some task that is adaptive but involves long-term learning instead of working memory training may be adequate. Several large-scale reviews and studies have suggested that working memory training sometimes improves performance on the working memory task that is trained, but does not generalize to reasoning tasks that must rely on working memory (in adults, Redick et al., 2013 , and Shipstead, Redick, & Engle, 2012 ; in children, Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2013 ). In somewhat of a contrast, other reviews suggest that the training of executive functions (inhibiting irrelevant information, updating working memory, controlling attention, etc.) does extend at least to tasks that use similar processes ( Diamond & Lee, 2011 ) and some basically concur also for working memory ( Chein & Morrison, 2010 ). So there is an ongoing controversy, even among those who have written meta-analyses and reviews of research.

One might ask how it is possible to improve working memory without having the effect of improving performance on other tasks that rely on working memory. This can happen because there are potentially two ways in which training can improve task performance. First, working memory training theoretically might increase the function of a basic process, much as a muscle can be strengthened through practice. (Or at least, individuals might learn that through diligent exertion of their attention and effort, they can do better.) That is presumably the route hoped for in training of working memory or executive function. Second, though, it is possible for working memory training to result in the discovery of a strategy for completing the task that is better than the strategy used initially. This can improve performance on the task being trained, but the experience and the strategy learned may well be irrelevant to performance on other educational tasks, even those that rely on working memory. This route might be expected if, as I suspect, participants typically look for a way to solve a problem that is not very attention-demanding, unless the payoff is high.

If there is successful working memory training, another issue is whether training is capable of producing super-normal performance or whether it is mostly capable of rectifying deficiencies. By way of analogy, consider physical exercise. If a person is already walking 6 miles a day, there might be little benefit to the heart of adding aerobic exercise. Similarly, if a person is already highly engaged in the environment and using attention control often and effectively during the day, there might be little benefit to the brain of adding working memory exercises. It remains quite conceivable, though, that such exercises are beneficial to certain individuals who are under-utilizing working memory. Nevertheless, as Diamond and Lee (2011) points out, there might be social or emotional reasons why this is the case and such factors would need to be addressed along with, or in some cases instead of, working memory training per se.

Working Memory and the Ultimate Goals of Education

What is the difference between learning and education? This is a question that has long been asked (for a history of the early period of educational psychology in the United States, for example, see Hall, 2003 ). Do children learn better when they are fed the information intensively, or allowed to explore the material? Should all children be expected to learn the same material, or should children be separated into different tracks and taught the information that is thought to help them the most in their own most likely future walks of life?

A fundamental difference between learning and education, many would agree, is that education should facilitate the acquisition of skills that will promote continued learning after the student leaves school. Of course, after the student leaves school, a major difference is that there is no teacher to decide what is to be learned, or how. Therefore, what seems to be most important, many would agree, is critical thinking skills. There is some sentiment that these skills can be trained (although for an opposing view see Tricot & Sweller, in press ). For example, Halpern (1998 p. 449) suggests the following emphases for training critical thinking: “(a) a dispositional component to prepare learners for efforiful cognitive work, (b) instruction in the skills of critical thinking, (c) training in the structural aspects of problems and arguments to promote transcontextual transfer of critical-thinking skills, and (d) a metacognitive component that includes checking for accuracy and monitoring progress toward the goal.” Although I could find few well-controlled, peer-reviewed studies supporting the notion that it is possible to train critical thinking skills, optimistic evidence is beginning to roll in. For example, Shim and Walczak (2012) found that professors asking challenging questions resulted in more improvement in both subjective and objective measures of critical thinking. The objective measure required that students clarify, analyze, evaluate, and extend arguments, and increased 0.55 standardized units for every 1-unit increase in the rating of challenging questions asked. The gain was much stronger in students with high pretest scores in critical thinking. Halpern et al. (2012) have designed a computerized module to train critical thinking skills and obtained very encouraging initial results, with well-controlled training experiments in progress according to the report.

One can then ask, to what extent is the training of these higher-level skills dependent on the student’s working memory ability? The association is likely to be substantial, given the high correlation between working memory and reasoning ability even among normal adults ( Kyllonen & Christal, 1990 ; Süβ, Oberauer, Wittmann, Wilhelm, & Schulze, 2002 ). There is the possibility that training working memory will in some way improve reasoning and vice versa, though most would agree at this point that the case has not yet been completely made (e.g., Jaeggi & Buschkuehl, 2013 ; Shipstead et al., 2012 ).

A current interest of mine is to understand how fallacies in reasoning might be related to fallacies in working memory performance. There appear to be some similarities between the two. One of the best-known reasoning fallacies is confirmation bias. In a key example ( Wason & Shapiro, 1971 ) participants are given a set of cards laid on the table, each having a letter on one side and a number on the other, and are asked which cards must be turned over to assess a rule (e.g., If a card has a vowel on one side, it has an even number on the other side ). Participants get that they must turn over the cards that can either confirm or disconfirm the rule (in the example, the cards showing vowels). They often fail to realize that they must also turn over cards that can only disconfirm the rule. In the example, one must turn over cards with odd numbers because the rule is disconfirmed if any of those cards have a vowel on the other side. In contrast, cards that can only confirm the rule are irrelevant. (One should not turn over cards with even numbers because the rule is technically not disconfirmed no matter whether there is a consonant or vowel on the other side.)

Chen and Cowan (in press) found performance on a working memory task that closely resembles confirmation bias. In one procedure, a spatial array of letters was presented on each trial, followed by a set of all of the letters at the bottom of the screen and a single location marked; the task was to select the correct letter for the marked location. In another procedure, the spatial array of letters was followed by a single letter from the array at the bottom of the screen and all of the locations marked; the task was to select the correct location for the presented letter. When working memory does not happen to contain the probed item, these procedures allow the use of disconfirming information. In the first task, for example, a participant might reason as follows: The letters were K, R, Q, and L. I know the locations of only R and L and neither of them match the probed location. Therefore, I know that the answer must be K or Q and I will guess randomly between them . That would be comparable to using disconfirming evidence. The pattern of data, however, did not appear to indicate that kind of process. Instead, participants answered correctly if they knew the probed item and otherwise guessed randomly among all of the other choices, without using the process of elimination. A mathematical model that assumed the latter process showed near-perfect convergence in capacity between the procedures described above and the usual change-detection procedure. If we instead assumed a mathematical model of performance in which disconfirming evidence was used through the process of elimination, there was no such convergence between the procedures.

So in reasoning and in working memory, processing tends to be inefficient, and it remains to be seen whether it can be meaningfully improved in terms of eliminating confirmation bias. Perhaps people with insufficient working memory or intelligence will always be stuck in such inefficient reasoning and there is nothing we can do. Arguing against that pessimistic view, however, is the recent finding ( Stanovich, West, & Toplak, 2013 ) that the tendency to evaluate evidence more favorably when it agrees with one’s own view occurs across the board and is not correlated with intelligence, and presumably therefore not correlated with working memory either. One might be able to train individuals to make the best use of the working memory they have without worrying about increasing the basic capacity of working memory, either by training critical thinking skills (Halpern, 1989) or by instilling expertise ( Eriksson et al., 2004 ).

Working memory is the retention of a small amount of information in a readily accessible form, which facilitates planning, comprehension, reasoning, and problem-solving. When we talk of working memory, we often include not only the memory itself, but also the executive control skills that are used to manage information in working memory and the cognitive processing of information. Theoretically, there is still uncertainty about the basic limitations on working memory: are they limitations on concurrent holding capacity, mnemonic processing speed, duration of retention of information before it decays, or just the same sorts of interference properties that apply to long-term memory? While these basic issues are debated and empirical investigations continue, there is much greater agreement about what results are obtained in particular test circumstances; the results of working memory studies seem rather replicable, but small differences in method produce large differences in results, so that one cannot assume that a particular working memory finding is highly generalizable.

For learning and education, it is important to take into account the basic principles of cognitive development and cognitive psychology, adjusting the materials to the working memory capabilities of the learner. We are not yet at a point at which every task can be analyzed in advance in order to predict which tasks are doable with a particular working memory capability. It is possible, though, to monitor performance and keep in mind that failure could be due to working memory limitations, adjusting the presentation accordingly. Keeping in mind the limitations of working memory of listeners and readers could easily help to improve one’s lecturing and writing styles. I hope that awareness of working memory leads to a world in which we are all more tolerant of one another’s inability to understand perfectly, are more humble and less arrogant, and are better able to communicate, educate one another, and reach common ground.

Acknowledgment

This work was completed with support from NIH grant R01-HD21338.

  • Andrews G, Halford GS. A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology. 2002; 45 :153–219. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM. Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence KW, Spence JT, editors. The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press; 1968. pp. 89–195. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baddeley AD. Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press; 1986. Oxford Psychology Series #11. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2000; 4 :417–423. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baddeley AD, Gathercole SE, Papagno C. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review. 1998; 105 :158–173. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barrouillet P, Gavens N, Vergauwe E, Gaillard V, Camos V. Working memory span development: A time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology. 2009; 45 :477–490. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baddeley AD, Hitch G. In: Working memory. Bower GH, editor. Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press; 1974. pp. 47–89. The psychology of learning and motivation. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baddeley A, Papagno C, Vallar G. When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language. 1988; 27 :586–595. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barrouillet P, Portrat S, Camos V. On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. Psychological Review. 2011; 118 :175–192. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bauer PJ, Fivush R, editors. Handbook on the development of children’s memory. Wiley-Blackwell; in press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bolton TL. The growth of memory in school children. American Journal of Psychology. 1892; 4 :362–380. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Borst G, Niven E, Logie RH. Visual mental image generation does not overlap with visual short-term memory: A dual-task interference study. Memory & cognition. 2012; 40 :360–372. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Broadbent DE. Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon Press; 1958. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown GDA, Neath I, Chater N. A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review. 2007; 114 :539–576. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burgess N, Hitch GJ. Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review. 1999; 106 :551–581. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Camos V, Barrouillet P. Developmental change in working memory strategies: From passive maintenance to active refreshing. Developmental Psychology. 2011; 47 :898–904. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Camos V, Mora G, Oberauer K. Adaptive choice between articulatory rehearsal and attentional refreshing in verbal working memory. Memory & Cognition. 2011; 39 :231–244. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Case R, Kurland DM, Goldberg J. Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1982; 33 :386–404. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chein J, Morrison A. Expanding the mind's workspace: training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2010; 17 :193–199. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen Z, Cowan N. Core verbal working memory capacity: The limit in words retained without covert articulation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2009; 62 :1420–1429. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen Z, Cowan N. Working memory inefficiency: Minimal information is utilized in visual recognition tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 2013; 39 :1449–1462. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chi MTH. In: Knowledge structures and memory development. Siegler R, editor. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. Children(s thinking: What develops? [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clark EV, Garnica OK. Is he coming or going? On the acquisition of deictic verbs. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1974; 13 :559–572. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cocchini G, Logie RH, Della Sala S, MacPherson SE, Baddeley AD. Concurrent performance of two memory tasks: Evidence for domain-specific working memory systems. Memory & Cognition. 2002; 30 :1086–1095. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Conrad R. Acoustic confusion in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology. 1964; 55 :75–84. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Courage ML, Cowan N, editors. The development of memory in infancy and childhood. Hove, U.K: Psychology Press; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin. 1988; 104 :163–191. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. Verbal memory span and the timing of spoken recall. Journal of Memory and Language. 1992; 31 :668–684. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. Attention and memory: An integrated framework. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995. Oxford Psychology Series, No. 26. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. An embedded-processes model of working memory. In: Miyake A, Shah P, editors. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, U.K: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 62–101. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2001; 24 :87–185. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. Working memory capacity. Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010; 19 :51–57. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N. The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks: Making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia. 2011; 49 :1401–1406. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, AuBuchon AM, Gilchrist AL, Ricker TJ, Saults JS. Age differences in visual working memory capacity: Not based on encoding limitations. Developmental Science. 2011; 14 :1066–1074. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Donnell K, Saults JS. A list-length constraint on incidental item-to-item associations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. in press [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Li D, Moffitt A, Becker TM, Martin EA, Saults JS, Christ SE. A neural region of abstract working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011; 23 :2852–2863. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Morey CC. How can dual-task working memory retention limits be investigated? Psychological Science. 2007; 18 :686–688. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Morey CC, AuBuchon AM, Zwilling CE, Gilchrist AL. Seven-year-olds allocate attention like adults unless working memory is overloaded. Developmental Science. 2010; 13 :120–133. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Nugent LD, Elliott EM, Ponomarev I, Saults JS. The role of attention in the development of short-term memory: Age differences in the verbal span of apprehension. Child Development. 1999; 70 :1082–1097. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Rouder JN, Blume CL, Saults JS. Models of verbal working memory capacity: What does it take to make them work? Psychological Review. 2012; 119 :480–499. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowan N, Saults JS, Winterowd C, Sherk M. Enhancement of 4-year-old children's memory span for phonologically similar and dissimilar word lists. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1991; 51 :30–52. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Craik FIM, Watkins MJ. The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1973; 12 :599–607. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior. 1980; 19 :450–466. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011; 333 :959–964. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ebbinghaus H. In: Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. Ruger HA, Bussenius CE, translators. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University; 1885/1913. (Originally in German, Ueber das gedächtnis: Untersuchen zur experimentellen psychologie ) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway ARA. Working memory, short term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1999; 128 :309–331. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ericsson KA, Chase WG, Faloon S. Acquisition of a memory skill. Science. 1980; 208 :1181–1182. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ericsson KA, Delaney PF, Weaver G, Mahadevan R. Uncovering the structure of a memorist’s superior basic-memory capacity. Cognitive Psychology. 2004; 49 :191–237. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ericsson KA, Kintsch W. Long-term working-memory. Psychological Review. 1995; 102 :211–245. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farrell S, Lewandowsky S. An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2002; 9 :59–79. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Flavell JH, Beach DH, Chinsky JM. Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Development. 1966; 37 :283–299. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fougnie D, Marois R. What limits working memory capacity? Evidence for modality-specific sources to the simultaneous storage of visual and auditory arrays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2011; 37 :1329–1341. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Garcia L, Nussbaum M, Preiss DD. Is the use of information and communication technology related to performance in working memory tasks? Evidence from seventh-grade students. Computers & Education. 2011; 57 :2068–2076. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Garrity LI. An electromyographical study of subvocal speech and recall in preschool children. Developmental Psychology. 1975; 11 :274–281. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language. 1989; 28 :200–213. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language. 1990; 29 :336–360. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gathercole SE, Lamont E, Alloway TP. Working memory in the classroom. In: Pickering SJ, editor. Working memory and education. San Diego: Academic Press; 2006. pp. 219–240. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H. The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology. 2004; 40 :177–190. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Geary DC. An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist. 2008; 43 :179–195. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gelman R. Accessing one-to-one correspondence: still another paper about conservation. British Journal of Psychology. 1982; 73 :209–220. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gilchrist AL, Cowan N, Naveh-Benjamin M. Investigating the childhood development of working memory using sentences: New evidence for the growth of chunk capacity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2009; 104 :252–265. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halford GS, Cowan N, Andrews G. Separating cognitive capacity from knowledge: A new hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007; 11 :236–242. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halford GS, Wilson WH, Phillips S. Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1998; 21 :803–865. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hall VC. Educational psychology from 1890 to 1920. In: Zimmerman BJ, Schunk DH, editors. Educational psychology: A century of contributions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halpern DF. Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacogitive monitoring. American Psychologist. 1998; 53 :449–455. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halpern DF, Millis K, Graesser AC, Butler H, Forsyth C, Cai Z. Operation ARA: A computerized learning game that teaches critical thinking and scientific reasoning. Thinking Skills And Creativity. 2012; 7 :93–100. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hebb DO. Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley; 1949. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Henry LA. The effects of word length and phonemic similarity in young children's short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1991; 43A :35–52. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Henson RNA. Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model. Cognitive Psychology. 1998; 36 :73–137. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hulme C, Tordoff V. Working memory development: The effects of speech rate, word length, and acoustic similarity on serial recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1989; 47 :72–87. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M. Training working memory. In: Alloway TP, Alloway RG, editors. Working memory: The connected intelligence. NY: Psychology Press; 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
  • James W. The principles of psychology. NY: Henry Holt; 1890. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jarrold C, Citroën R. Reevaluating key evidence for the development of rehearsal: Phonological similarity effects in children are subject to proportional scaling artifacts. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 :837–847. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jevons WS. The power of numerical discrimination. Nature. 1871; 3 :281–282. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jolicoeur P, Dell'Acqua R. The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology. 1998; 36 :138–202. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kail R. The development of memory in children. 3rd edition. NY: W.H.Freeman; 1990. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kail R, Salthouse TA. Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica. 1994; 86 :199–255. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kane MJ, Brown LH, McVay JC, Silvia PJ, Myin-Germeys I, Kwapil TR. For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science. 2007; 18 :614–621. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2003; 132 :47–70. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The generality of workingmemory capacity: A latentvariable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004; 133 :189–217. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010; 14 :317–324. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kyllonen PC, Christal RE. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence. 1990; 14 :389–433. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kyndt E, Cascallar E, Dochy F. Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Attention, and Their Relationship with Students' Approaches to Learning. Higher Education: The International Journal Of Higher Education And Educational Planning. 2012; 64 :285–297. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lewandowsky S, Duncan M, Brown GDA. Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2004; 11 :771–790. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Locke J. An essay concerning human understanding. London: Thomas Bassett; 1690. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Logie RH. The seven ages of working memory. In: Richardson JTE, Engle RW, Hasher L, Logie RH, Stoltzfus ER, Zacks RT, editors. Working memory and human cognition. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996. pp. 31–65. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Logie RH, Van Der Meulen M. Fragmenting and integrating visuospatial working memory. In: Brockmole JR, editor. Representing the visual world in memory. Hove, U.K: Psychology Press; 2009. pp. 1–32. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luck SJ, Vogel EK. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature. 1997; 390 :279–281. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mandler G, Shebo BJ. Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 1982; 111 :1–22. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 :270–291. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller GA. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review. 1956; 63 :81–97. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miller GA, Galanter E, Pribram KH. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc; 1960. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morey CC, Bieler M. Visual short-term memory always requires attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2013; 20 :163–170. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murdock BB. A distributed memory model for serial-order information. Psychological Review. 1983; 90 :316–338. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murdock BB, Walker KD. Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Leaning and Verbal Behavior. 1969; 8 :665–676. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nelson KJ. Variations in children's concepts by age and category. Child Development. 1974; 45 :577–584. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norman DA. Toward a theory of memory and attention. Psychological Review. 1968; 75 (6):522–536. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oberauer K, Lewandowsky S. Forgetting in immediate serial recall: decay, temporal distinctiveness, or interference? Psychological Review. 2008; 115 :544–576. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oberauer K, Lewandowsky S. Modeling working memory: A computational implementation of the time-based resource-sharing theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2011; 18 :10–45. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oberauer K, Lewandowsky S, Farrell S, Jarrold C, Greaves M. Modeling working memory: An interference model of complex span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2012; 19 :779–819. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ornstein PA, Naus MJ. Rehearsal processes in children's memory. In: Ornstein PA, editor. Memory development in children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. pp. 69–99. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Paas F, Sweller J. An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: Using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review. 2012; 24 :27–45. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pascual-Leone J, Johnson J. A developmental theory of mental attention: Its applications to measurement and task analysis. In: Barrouillet P, Gaillard V, editors. Cognitive development and working Memory: From neoPiagetian to cognitive approaches. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 2011. pp. 13–46. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pascual-Leone J, Smith J. The encoding and decoding of symbols by children: A new experimental paradigm and a neo-Piagetian model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1969; 8 :328–355. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Penney CG. Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory & Cognition. 1989; 17 :398–422. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pickering SJ. Working memory and education. San Diego: Academic Press; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Redick TS, Shipstead Z, Harrison TL, Hicks KL, Fried DE, Hambrick DZ, Kane MJ, Engle RW. No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2013; 142 :359–379. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ricker TJ, Cowan N. Loss of visual working memory within seconds: The combined use of refreshable and non-refreshable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2010; 36 :1355–1368. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ricker TJ, Cowan N. Differences between presentation methods in working memory procedures: A matter of working memory consolidation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. in press [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rouder JN, Morey RD, Cowan N, Zwilling CE, Morey CC, Pratte MS. An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS) 2008; 105 :5975–5979. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sabol TJ, Pianta RC. Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and socioemotional development at the end of elementary school. Child Development. 2012; 83 :282–299. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saltz E, Soller E, Sigel IE. The development of natural language concepts. Child Development. 1972; 43 :1191–1202. 1972. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schüler A, Scheiter K, Genuchten E. The Role of Working Memory in Multimedia Instruction: Is Working Memory Working During Learning from Text and Pictures? Educational Psychology Review. 2011; 23 :389–411. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shim W, Walczak K. The Impact of Faculty Teaching Practices on the Development of Students' Critical Thinking Skills. International Journal Of Teaching And Learning In Higher Education. 2012; 24 :16–30. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin. 2012; 138 :628–654. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Slevc L. Saying What's on Your Mind: Working Memory Effects on Sentence Production. Journal Of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, And Cognition. 2011; 37 :1503–1514. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sokolov EN. Perception and the conditioned reflex. NY: Pergamon Press; 1963. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stanovich KE, West RF, Toplak ME. Myside Bias, Rational Thinking, and Intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2013; 22 :259–264. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Süβ HM, Oberauer K, Wittmann WW, Wilhelm O, Schulze R. Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability—and a little bit more. Intelligence. 2002; 30 :261–288. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sweller J. Cognitive load theory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 2011; 55 :37–76. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sweller J, van Merrienboer JJG, Paas FGWC. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review. 1998; 10 :251–296. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tan L, Ward G. A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2000; 26 :1589–1625. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Todd JJ, Marois R. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature. 2004; 428 :751–754. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Treisman AM. Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1960; 12 :242–248. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tricot A, Sweller J. Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review. in press [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Der Meulen M, Logie RH, Sala SD. Selective interference with image retention and generation: Evidence for the workspace model. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2009; 62 :1568–1580. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vergauwe E, Barrouillet P, Camos V. Do mental processes share a domain-general resource? Psychological Science. 2010; 21 :384–390. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vogel EK, McCollough AW, Machizawa MG. Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature. 2005; 438 :500–503. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wason PC, Shapiro D. Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1971; 23 :63–71. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilding J. Over the top: Are there exceptions to the basic capacity limit? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2001; 24 :152–153. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilhelm O, Engle RW, editors. Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence. London: Sage; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wundt W. In: Lectures on human and animal psychology. Creighton JE, Titchener EB, editors. Bristol, U.K: Thoemmes Press; 1894/1998. Translated from the second German. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Xu Y, Chun MM. Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature. 2006; 440 :91–95. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang W, Luck SJ. Sudden death and gradual decay in visual working memory. Psychological Science. 2009; 20 :423–428. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

Title: A machine learning perspective of the impact of COVID-19 on the Indian stock market

Authors : Jared Dominic Fernandez; Arya Kumar

Addresses : Department of Economics and Finance, Birla Institute of Technology and Science (BITS), Pilani 333031, Rajasthan, India ' Department of Economics and Finance, Birla Institute of Technology and Science (BITS), Pilani 333031, Rajasthan, India

Abstract : Stock markets across the globe were affected by the outburst of COVID-19 in early 2020. This attracted researchers to analyse and understand the implications of such sudden happenings on stock prices, more so by application of latest methodologies that are slowly finding greater relevance in social sciences. This study uses econometric and machine learning techniques to measure the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic and predict the future trend of the stock market in India. This paper attempts to examine the reliability of traditional methods and machine learning techniques to establish their relevance in predicting stock market trends. The study also uses variable perturbation and least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) to identify which variables have more significant predictive weightage in the machine learning models. The study reveals that machine learning models outperform econometric models in their predictive power amidst more significant uncertainty. Moreover, a gated recurrent unit (GRU) model is able to capture the stock market dip and gradual recovery much better than a long short-term memory (LSTM) model. The findings of the study reveal that the number of cases and deaths had a significant impact on stock prices and predictive ability to forecast the NIFTY close price.

Keywords : stock market prediction; machine learning; long short-term memory; LSTM; gated recurrent unit; GRU; variable perturbation; COVID-19; NIFTY; Indian stock market.

DOI : 10.1504/IJBISE.2024.139145

International Journal of Business Intelligence and Systems Engineering, 2024 Vol.2 No.1, pp.1 - 22

Received: 10 Mar 2022 Received in revised form: 02 Nov 2022 Accepted: 09 Nov 2022 Published online: 15 Jun 2024 *

Keep up-to-date

  • Our Newsletter ( subscribe for free )
  • New issue alerts
  • Inderscience is a member of publishing organisations including:

CLOCKSS

COMMENTS

  1. Focus on learning and memory

    In this special issue of Nature Neuroscience, we feature an assortment of reviews and perspectives that explore the topic of learning and memory. Learning new information and skills, storing this ...

  2. Cognitive neuroscience perspective on memory: overview and summary

    This paper explores memory from a cognitive neuroscience perspective and examines associated neural mechanisms. It examines the different types of memory: working, declarative, and non-declarative, and the brain regions involved in each type. The paper highlights the role of different brain regions, such as the prefrontal cortex in working ...

  3. Learning and memory

    The brain is the organ that is responsible for what we call the mind. It is the basis for thinking, feeling, wanting, perceiving, learning and memory, curiosity, and behavior. Memory is a fundamental mental process, and without memory we are capable of nothing but simple reflexes and stereotyped behaviors. Thus, learning and memory is one of ...

  4. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition ® publishes original experimental and theoretical research on human cognition, with a special emphasis on learning, memory, language, and higher cognition.. The journal publishes impactful articles of any length, including literature reviews, meta-analyses, replications, theoretical notes, and commentaries on previously ...

  5. Memory: Neurobiological mechanisms and assessment

    NEUROBIOLOGY. Research suggests that the hippocampus plays an important role in memory consolidation. It was proved by experiments, wherein lesions were applied to rat hippocampi at various times after learning.[] Looking from the molecular and cellular point of view two proteins involved are calcium calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII)[6,7] and protein phosphatase 1 (PP1).

  6. Learning and memory

    RSS Feed. Learning and memory refers to the processes of acquiring, retaining and retrieving information in the central nervous system. It consists of forming stable long-term memories that ...

  7. Learning and memory

    Deep brain stimulation during sleep enhances human brain synchrony and memory. Sleep helps to stabilize long-term memories, possibly through the temporal synchronization of neuronal activity in ...

  8. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    We are especially interested in manuscripts that examine the neural circuits and molecular mechanisms underlying learning, memory and plasticity in both experimental animals and human subjects. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory includes major research and review papers as well as rapid communications reporting new results.

  9. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition

    The Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (JARMAC) publishes the highest-quality applied research in memory and cognition, in the format of empirical reports, review articles, and target papers with invited peer commentary. The goal of this unique journal is to reach psychological scientists and other researchers working in this field and related areas, as well as professionals ...

  10. Learning and memory

    The main goals of research with NBS in learning and memory have been to: (1) identify underlying neuropsychological processes and neurobiological components; (2) find out how this knowledge can be used to diagnose and restore dysfunctions of learning and memory in various patient populations; and (3) assess the use of NBS for enhancement ...

  11. Frontiers

    Recent studies using functional neuroimaging techniques detect and recognize human emotional states and have become a topic of increasing research in cognitive neuroscience, affective neuroscience, and educational psychology to optimize learning and memory outcomes (Carew and Magsamen, 2010; Um et al., 2012). Human emotions comprise complex ...

  12. Cognitive-Load Theory: Methods to Manage Working Memory Load in the

    The main goal of cognitive-load theory (CLT; Paas, Renkl, & Sweller, 2003; Sweller, Ayres, & Kalyuga, 2011; Sweller, van Merrienboer, & Paas, 1998; Sweller, van Merriënboer, & Paas, 2019) is to optimize learning of complex cognitive tasks by transforming contemporary scientific knowledge on the manner in which cognitive structures and processes are organized (i.e., cognitive architecture ...

  13. Why Writing by Hand Is Better for Memory and Learning

    A recent study in Frontiers in Psychology monitored brain activity in students taking notes and found that those writing by hand had higher levels of electrical activity across a wide range of ...

  14. Learning and memory under stress: implications for the classroom

    For example, stress during learning specifically enhanced memory for material that was related to the context of the stressful task and thus putatively more relevant. 20 Material that is unrelated ...

  15. Learning and Memory 2023

    The 2023 International Conference on Learning and Memory (#LEARNMEM2023) is held on April 26-30, 2023 to celebrate the field's accomplishments and chart the path for the next fifty years.This Research Topic serves as a repository of the discoveries and advances featured at the conference. In this Research Topic, we welcome papers on ...

  16. Learning and memory

    Learning is the acquisition of skill or knowledge, while memory is the expression of what you've acquired. Another difference is the speed with which the two things happen. If you acquire the new skill or knowledge slowly and laboriously, that's learning. If acquisition occurs instantly, that's making a memory.

  17. Evidence-Based Strategies to Improve Memory and Learning

    This gives one the impression of mastery of a subject because the text becomes familiar. In contrast, research indicates that this study method involves short-term memory rather than deep learning. A more beneficial study strategy that is supported by research is retrieval practice, which involves recalling information from memory.

  18. (PDF) A REVIEW ON LEARNING AND MEMORY

    Another definition Learning refers to a more or. less permanent change in behavior which occurs as a. result of practice, is a little better. Learning and memory. are vital attributes of human ...

  19. The consolidation of newly learned movements depends upon somatosensory

    The findings are consistent with the S1 participation in the encoding of learning related changes to movements and in the retention of human motor memory. Significance Statement This paper uses transcranial magnetic stimulation which is applied after sensorimotor learning to disrupt brain areas that might be involved in the retention of newly ...

  20. [2305.14314] QLoRA: Efficient Finetuning of Quantized LLMs

    We present QLoRA, an efficient finetuning approach that reduces memory usage enough to finetune a 65B parameter model on a single 48GB GPU while preserving full 16-bit finetuning task performance. QLoRA backpropagates gradients through a frozen, 4-bit quantized pretrained language model into Low Rank Adapters~(LoRA). Our best model family, which we name Guanaco, outperforms all previous openly ...

  21. Diagnostics

    Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a neurological disorder that significantly impairs cognitive function, leading to memory loss and eventually death. AD progresses through three stages: early stage, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (middle stage), and dementia. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease is crucial and can improve survival rates among patients. Traditional methods for diagnosing AD ...

  22. Focus on Learning and Memory

    Focus on Learning and Memory. Learning new information and skills, storing memories of this knowledge, and retrieving, modifying, or forgetting these memories over time are critical for flexibly ...

  23. People

    At IBM Research, we're inventing what's next in AI, quantum computing, and hybrid cloud to shape the world ahead.

  24. Memory: from the laboratory to everyday life

    Abstract. One of the key goals of memory research is to develop a basic understanding of the nature and characteristics of memory processes and systems. Another important goal is to develop useful applications of basic research to everyday life. This editorial considers two lines of work that illustrate some of the prospects for applying memory ...

  25. Six Books That Explain The History And Meaning Of Juneteenth

    The essays in her book invite readers to enter a world shaped by the forces of freedom and slavery. Reed's exploration of the history and legacy of Juneteenth is a poignant reminder of the hard history all Americans face. 'O Freedom! Afro-American Emancipation Celebrations' William H. Wiggins Jr.'s "O Freedom!

  26. Browse journals and books

    Browse Calls for Papers. Browse 5,060 journals and 35,600 books. A; A Review on Diverse Neurological Disorders. ... The Nuclear Research Foundation School Certificate Integrated, Volume 1. Book • 1966. ... A Conversation on New Approaches to Teaching and Learning in the post-COVID World. Book • 2022. ACC Current Journal Review. Journal

  27. The memory function of sleep

    The first evidence for a causal role of re-activation during SWS in memory consolidation came from a study in humans learning spatial locations in the presence of an odour 15.Re-exposure to the ...

  28. The state of AI in 2023: Generative AI's breakout year

    In the past year, organizations using AI most often hired data engineers, machine learning engineers, and Al data scientists—all roles that respondents commonly reported hiring in the previous survey. ... About the research. The online survey was in the field April 11 to 21, 2023, and garnered responses from 1,684 participants representing ...

  29. Working Memory Underpins Cognitive Development, Learning, and Education

    Working memory is the retention of a small amount of information in a readily accessible form. It facilitates planning, comprehension, reasoning, and problem-solving. I examine the historical roots and conceptual development of the concept and the theoretical and practical implications of current debates about working memory mechanisms.

  30. Article: A machine learning perspective of the impact of COVID-19 on

    This study uses econometric and machine learning techniques to measure the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic and predict the future trend of the stock market in India. This paper attempts to examine the reliability of traditional methods and machine learning techniques to establish their relevance in predicting stock market trends.